Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence
Debats N, Heuer H (2018)
Scientific Reports 8(1): 9796.
Zeitschriftenaufsatz
| Veröffentlicht | Englisch
Download
s41598-018-28251-x.debats.pdf
3.07 MB
Autor*in
Einrichtung
Abstract / Bemerkung
The brain needs to identify redundant sensory signals in order to integrate them optimally. The identification process, referred to as causal inference, depends on the spatial and temporal correspondence of the incoming sensory signals (‘online sensory causality evidence’) as well as on prior expectations regarding their causal relation. We here examine whether the same causal inference process underlies spatial integration of actions and their visual consequences. We used a basic cursor-control task for which online sensory causality evidence is provided by the correlated hand and cursor movements, and prior expectations are formed by everyday experience of such correlated movements. Participants made out-and-back movements and subsequently judged the hand or cursor movement endpoints. In one condition, we omitted the online sensory causality evidence by showing the cursor only at the movement endpoint. The integration strength was lower than in conditions where the cursor was visible during the outward movement, but a substantial level of integration persisted. These findings support the hypothesis that the binding of actions and their visual consequences is based on the general mechanism of optimal integration, and they specifically show that such binding can occur even if it is previous experience only that identifies the action consequence.
Erscheinungsjahr
2018
Zeitschriftentitel
Scientific Reports
Band
8
Ausgabe
1
Art.-Nr.
9796
Urheberrecht / Lizenzen
ISSN
2045-2322
eISSN
2045-2322
Finanzierungs-Informationen
Open-Access-Publikationskosten wurden durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft und die Universität Bielefeld gefördert.
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2921023
Zitieren
Debats N, Heuer H. Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence. Scientific Reports. 2018;8(1): 9796.
Debats, N., & Heuer, H. (2018). Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 9796. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-28251-x
Debats, Nienke, and Heuer, Herbert. 2018. “Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence”. Scientific Reports 8 (1): 9796.
Debats, N., and Heuer, H. (2018). Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence. Scientific Reports 8:9796.
Debats, N., & Heuer, H., 2018. Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence. Scientific Reports, 8(1): 9796.
N. Debats and H. Heuer, “Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence”, Scientific Reports, vol. 8, 2018, : 9796.
Debats, N., Heuer, H.: Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence. Scientific Reports. 8, : 9796 (2018).
Debats, Nienke, and Heuer, Herbert. “Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence”. Scientific Reports 8.1 (2018): 9796.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Creative Commons Namensnennung 4.0 International Public License (CC-BY 4.0):
Volltext(e)
Name
s41598-018-28251-x.debats.pdf
3.07 MB
Access Level
Open Access
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-06T09:19:00Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
689c55fe88d32b1b3d8ea1bc1534ff14
Daten bereitgestellt von European Bioinformatics Institute (EBI)
2 Zitationen in Europe PMC
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Effects of Hand and Hemispace on Multisensory Integration of Hand Position and Visual Feedback.
Rand MK, Heuer H., Front Psychol 10(), 2019
PMID: 30809172
Rand MK, Heuer H., Front Psychol 10(), 2019
PMID: 30809172
Effects of auditory feedback on movements with two-segment sequence and eye-hand coordination.
Rand MK., Exp Brain Res 236(12), 2018
PMID: 30159590
Rand MK., Exp Brain Res 236(12), 2018
PMID: 30159590
45 References
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion.
Ernst MO, Banks MS., Nature 415(6870), 2002
PMID: 11807554
Ernst MO, Banks MS., Nature 415(6870), 2002
PMID: 11807554
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Bayesian integration of spatial information.
Cheng K, Shettleworth SJ, Huttenlocher J, Rieser JJ., Psychol Bull 133(4), 2007
PMID: 17592958
Cheng K, Shettleworth SJ, Huttenlocher J, Rieser JJ., Psychol Bull 133(4), 2007
PMID: 17592958
Object perception as Bayesian inference.
Kersten D, Mamassian P, Yuille A., Annu Rev Psychol 55(), 2004
PMID: 14744217
Kersten D, Mamassian P, Yuille A., Annu Rev Psychol 55(), 2004
PMID: 14744217
Sound-induced flash illusion as an optimal percept.
Shams L, Ma WJ, Beierholm U., Neuroreport 16(17), 2005
PMID: 16272880
Shams L, Ma WJ, Beierholm U., Neuroreport 16(17), 2005
PMID: 16272880
Multisensory integration, perception and ecological validity.
De Gelder B, Bertelson P., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 7(10), 2003
PMID: 14550494
De Gelder B, Bertelson P., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 7(10), 2003
PMID: 14550494
Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy.
Welch RB, Warren DH., Psychol Bull 88(3), 1980
PMID: 7003641
Welch RB, Warren DH., Psychol Bull 88(3), 1980
PMID: 7003641
Resolving multisensory conflict: a strategy for balancing the costs and benefits of audio-visual integration.
Roach NW, Heron J, McGraw PV., Proc. Biol. Sci. 273(1598), 2006
PMID: 16901835
Roach NW, Heron J, McGraw PV., Proc. Biol. Sci. 273(1598), 2006
PMID: 16901835
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Causal inference in perception.
Shams L, Beierholm UR., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 14(9), 2010
PMID: 20705502
Shams L, Beierholm UR., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 14(9), 2010
PMID: 20705502
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Vision and touch are automatically integrated for the perception of sequences of events.
Bresciani JP, Dammeier F, Ernst MO., J Vis 6(5), 2006
PMID: 16881788
Bresciani JP, Dammeier F, Ernst MO., J Vis 6(5), 2006
PMID: 16881788
Causal inference in multisensory perception.
Kording KP, Beierholm U, Ma WJ, Quartz S, Tenenbaum JB, Shams L., PLoS ONE 2(9), 2007
PMID: 17895984
Kording KP, Beierholm U, Ma WJ, Quartz S, Tenenbaum JB, Shams L., PLoS ONE 2(9), 2007
PMID: 17895984
Sensory reliability shapes perceptual inference via two mechanisms.
Rohe T, Noppeney U., J Vis 15(5), 2015
PMID: 26067540
Rohe T, Noppeney U., J Vis 15(5), 2015
PMID: 26067540
Probability matching as a computational strategy used in perception.
Wozny DR, Beierholm UR, Shams L., PLoS Comput. Biol. 6(8), 2010
PMID: 20700493
Wozny DR, Beierholm UR, Shams L., PLoS Comput. Biol. 6(8), 2010
PMID: 20700493
Human trimodal perception follows optimal statistical inference.
Wozny DR, Beierholm UR, Shams L., J Vis 8(3), 2008
PMID: 18484830
Wozny DR, Beierholm UR, Shams L., J Vis 8(3), 2008
PMID: 18484830
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Temporal and spatial dependency of the ventriloquism effect.
Slutsky DA, Recanzone GH., Neuroreport 12(1), 2001
PMID: 11201094
Slutsky DA, Recanzone GH., Neuroreport 12(1), 2001
PMID: 11201094
The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity.
Gepshtein S, Burge J, Ernst MO, Banks MS., J Vis 5(11), 2005
PMID: 16441199
Gepshtein S, Burge J, Ernst MO, Banks MS., J Vis 5(11), 2005
PMID: 16441199
When correlation implies causation in multisensory integration.
Parise CV, Spence C, Ernst MO., Curr. Biol. 22(1), 2011
PMID: 22177899
Parise CV, Spence C, Ernst MO., Curr. Biol. 22(1), 2011
PMID: 22177899
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch.
Ernst MO., J Vis 7(5), 2007
PMID: 18217847
Ernst MO., J Vis 7(5), 2007
PMID: 18217847
A dedicated binding mechanism for the visual control of movement.
Reichenbach A, Franklin DW, Zatka-Haas P, Diedrichsen J., Curr. Biol. 24(7), 2014
PMID: 24631246
Reichenbach A, Franklin DW, Zatka-Haas P, Diedrichsen J., Curr. Biol. 24(7), 2014
PMID: 24631246
Perceptual attraction in tool use: evidence for a reliability-based weighting mechanism.
Debats NB, Ernst MO, Heuer H., J. Neurophysiol. 117(4), 2017
PMID: 28100656
Debats NB, Ernst MO, Heuer H., J. Neurophysiol. 117(4), 2017
PMID: 28100656
Kinematic cross-correlation induces sensory integration across separate objects.
Debats NB, Ernst MO, Heuer H., Eur. J. Neurosci. 46(12), 2017
PMID: 29068094
Debats NB, Ernst MO, Heuer H., Eur. J. Neurosci. 46(12), 2017
PMID: 29068094
Implicit and explicit representations of hand position in tool use.
Rand MK, Heuer H., PLoS ONE 8(7), 2013
PMID: 23894307
Rand MK, Heuer H., PLoS ONE 8(7), 2013
PMID: 23894307
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Intra- and intermodal integration of discrepant visual and proprioceptive action effects.
Ladwig S, Sutter C, Musseler J., Exp Brain Res 231(4), 2013
PMID: 24101198
Ladwig S, Sutter C, Musseler J., Exp Brain Res 231(4), 2013
PMID: 24101198
Spatial action-effect binding.
Kirsch W, Pfister R, Kunde W., Atten Percept Psychophys 78(1), 2016
PMID: 26486641
Kirsch W, Pfister R, Kunde W., Atten Percept Psychophys 78(1), 2016
PMID: 26486641
Voluntary action and conscious awareness.
Haggard P, Clark S, Kalogeras J., Nat. Neurosci. 5(4), 2002
PMID: 11896397
Haggard P, Clark S, Kalogeras J., Nat. Neurosci. 5(4), 2002
PMID: 11896397
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Moving the weber fraction: the perceptual precision for moment of inertia increases with exploration force.
Debats NB, Kingma I, Beek PJ, Smeets JB., PLoS ONE 7(9), 2012
PMID: 23028437
Debats NB, Kingma I, Beek PJ, Smeets JB., PLoS ONE 7(9), 2012
PMID: 23028437
Recalibration of multisensory simultaneity: cross-modal transfer coincides with a change in perceptual latency
Di M, Machulla T-K, Ernst MO., 2009
Di M, Machulla T-K, Ernst MO., 2009
Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning.
Sober SJ, Sabes PN., Nat. Neurosci. 8(4), 2005
PMID: 15793578
Sober SJ, Sabes PN., Nat. Neurosci. 8(4), 2005
PMID: 15793578
Dissociating explicit and implicit measures of sensed hand position in tool use: Effect of relative frequency of judging different objects.
Rand MK, Heuer H., Atten Percept Psychophys 80(1), 2018
PMID: 29075991
Rand MK, Heuer H., Atten Percept Psychophys 80(1), 2018
PMID: 29075991
An internal model for sensorimotor integration.
Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI., Science 269(5232), 1995
PMID: 7569931
Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI., Science 269(5232), 1995
PMID: 7569931
Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning.
Kawato M., Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9(6), 1999
PMID: 10607637
Kawato M., Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9(6), 1999
PMID: 10607637
When knowing can replace seeing in audiovisual integration of actions.
Petrini K, Russell M, Pollick F., Cognition 110(3), 2009
PMID: 19121519
Petrini K, Russell M, Pollick F., Cognition 110(3), 2009
PMID: 19121519
Bayesian approaches to sensory integration for motor control.
Berniker M, Kording K., Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2(4), 2011
PMID: 26302201
Berniker M, Kording K., Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2(4), 2011
PMID: 26302201
Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning.
Kording KP, Wolpert DM., Nature 427(6971), 2004
PMID: 14724638
Kording KP, Wolpert DM., Nature 427(6971), 2004
PMID: 14724638
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Experience can change the 'light-from-above' prior.
Adams WJ, Graf EW, Ernst MO., Nat. Neurosci. 7(10), 2004
PMID: 15361877
Adams WJ, Graf EW, Ernst MO., Nat. Neurosci. 7(10), 2004
PMID: 15361877
Sensorimotor priors in nonstationary environments.
Narain D, van Beers RJ, Smeets JB, Brenner E., J. Neurophysiol. 109(5), 2012
PMID: 23235999
Narain D, van Beers RJ, Smeets JB, Brenner E., J. Neurophysiol. 109(5), 2012
PMID: 23235999
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Export
Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen
Web of Science
Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®Quellen
PMID: 29955156
PubMed | Europe PMC
Suchen in