Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults
Schubert JTW, Badde S, Röder B, Heed T (2017)
PLOS ONE 12(12): e0189067.
Zeitschriftenaufsatz
| Veröffentlicht | Englisch
Download
journal.pone.0189067.pdf
5.05 MB
Autor*in
Schubert, Jonathan T. W.;
Badde, Stephanie;
Röder, Brigitte;
Heed, TobiasUniBi
Einrichtung
Abstract / Bemerkung
Task demands modulate tactile localization in sighted humans, presumably through weight adjustments in the spatial integration of anatomical, skin-based, and external, posture-based information. In contrast, previous studies have suggested that congenitally blind humans, by default, refrain from automatic spatial integration and localize touch using only skin-based information. Here, sighted and congenitally blind participants localized tactile targets on the palm or back of one hand, while ignoring simultaneous tactile distractors at congruent or incongruent locations on the other hand. We probed the interplay of anatomical and external location codes for spatial congruency effects by varying hand posture: the palms either both faced down, or one faced down and one up. In the latter posture, externally congruent target and distractor locations were anatomically incongruent and vice versa. Target locations had to be reported either anatomically (“palm” or “back” of the hand), or externally (“up” or “down” in space). Under anatomical instructions, performance was more accurate for anatomically congruent than incongruent target-distractor pairs. In contrast, under external instructions, performance was more accurate for externally congruent than incongruent pairs. These modulations were evident in sighted and blind individuals. Notably, distractor effects were overall far smaller in blind than in sighted participants, despite comparable target-distractor identification performance. Thus, the absence of developmental vision seems to be associated with an increased ability to focus tactile attention towards a non-spatially defined target. Nevertheless, that blind individuals exhibited effects of hand posture and task instructions in their congruency effects suggests that, like the sighted, they automatically integrate anatomical and external information during tactile localization. Moreover, spatial integration in tactile processing is, thus, flexibly adapted by top-down information—here, task instruction—even in the absence of developmental vision.
Erscheinungsjahr
2017
Zeitschriftentitel
PLOS ONE
Band
12
Ausgabe
12
Art.-Nr.
e0189067
Urheberrecht / Lizenzen
ISSN
1932-6203
Finanzierungs-Informationen
Open-Access-Publikationskosten wurden durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft und die Universität Bielefeld gefördert.
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2915823
Zitieren
Schubert JTW, Badde S, Röder B, Heed T. Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults. PLOS ONE. 2017;12(12): e0189067.
Schubert, J. T. W., Badde, S., Röder, B., & Heed, T. (2017). Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults. PLOS ONE, 12(12), e0189067. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0189067
Schubert, Jonathan T. W., Badde, Stephanie, Röder, Brigitte, and Heed, Tobias. 2017. “Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults”. PLOS ONE 12 (12): e0189067.
Schubert, J. T. W., Badde, S., Röder, B., and Heed, T. (2017). Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults. PLOS ONE 12:e0189067.
Schubert, J.T.W., et al., 2017. Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults. PLOS ONE, 12(12): e0189067.
J.T.W. Schubert, et al., “Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults”, PLOS ONE, vol. 12, 2017, : e0189067.
Schubert, J.T.W., Badde, S., Röder, B., Heed, T.: Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults. PLOS ONE. 12, : e0189067 (2017).
Schubert, Jonathan T. W., Badde, Stephanie, Röder, Brigitte, and Heed, Tobias. “Task demands affect spatial reference frame weighting during tactile localization in sighted and congenitally blind adults”. PLOS ONE 12.12 (2017): e0189067.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication (CC0 1.0):
Volltext(e)
Name
journal.pone.0189067.pdf
5.05 MB
Access Level
Open Access
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-25T06:48:53Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
c2b8bbbdca8de9c93e56d69ce21e0b18
Daten bereitgestellt von European Bioinformatics Institute (EBI)
1 Zitation in Europe PMC
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Alpha-band oscillations reflect external spatial coding for tactile stimuli in sighted, but not in congenitally blind humans.
Schubert JTW, Buchholz VN, Föcker J, Engel AK, Röder B, Heed T., Sci Rep 9(1), 2019
PMID: 31239467
Schubert JTW, Buchholz VN, Föcker J, Engel AK, Röder B, Heed T., Sci Rep 9(1), 2019
PMID: 31239467
62 References
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration.
Alais D, Burr D., Curr. Biol. 14(3), 2004
PMID: 14761661
Alais D, Burr D., Curr. Biol. 14(3), 2004
PMID: 14761661
Multisensory integration: psychophysics, neurophysiology, and computation.
Angelaki DE, Gu Y, DeAngelis GC., Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19(4), 2009
PMID: 19616425
Angelaki DE, Gu Y, DeAngelis GC., Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19(4), 2009
PMID: 19616425
Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion.
Ernst MO, Banks MS., Nature 415(6870), 2002
PMID: 11807554
Ernst MO, Banks MS., Nature 415(6870), 2002
PMID: 11807554
A computational perspective on the neural basis of multisensory spatial representations.
Pouget A, Deneve S, Duhamel JR., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3(9), 2002
PMID: 12209122
Pouget A, Deneve S, Duhamel JR., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3(9), 2002
PMID: 12209122
Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning.
Sober SJ, Sabes PN., Nat. Neurosci. 8(4), 2005
PMID: 15793578
Sober SJ, Sabes PN., Nat. Neurosci. 8(4), 2005
PMID: 15793578
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2011
Consolidating behavioral and neurophysiologic findings to explain the influence of contextual interference during motor sequence learning.
Wright D, Verwey W, Buchanen J, Chen J, Rhee J, Immink M., Psychon Bull Rev 23(1), 2016
PMID: 26084879
Wright D, Verwey W, Buchanen J, Chen J, Rhee J, Immink M., Psychon Bull Rev 23(1), 2016
PMID: 26084879
Flexibly weighted integration of tactile reference frames.
Badde S, Roder B, Heed T., Neuropsychologia 70(), 2014
PMID: 25447059
Badde S, Roder B, Heed T., Neuropsychologia 70(), 2014
PMID: 25447059
Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 1937
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 1937
Multisensory processing in spatial orientation: an inverse probabilistic approach.
Clemens IA, De Vrijer M, Selen LP, Van Gisbergen JA, Medendorp WP., J. Neurosci. 31(14), 2011
PMID: 21471371
Clemens IA, De Vrijer M, Selen LP, Van Gisbergen JA, Medendorp WP., J. Neurosci. 31(14), 2011
PMID: 21471371
Cross-modal links in spatial attention.
Driver J, Spence C., Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 353(1373), 1998
PMID: 9770225
Driver J, Spence C., Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 353(1373), 1998
PMID: 9770225
Tactile remapping: from coordinate transformation to integration in sensorimotor processing.
Heed T, Buchholz VN, Engel AK, Roder B., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 19(5), 2015
PMID: 25843541
Heed T, Buchholz VN, Engel AK, Roder B., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 19(5), 2015
PMID: 25843541
The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space.
Holmes NP, Spence C., Cogn Process 5(2), 2004
PMID: 16467906
Holmes NP, Spence C., Cogn Process 5(2), 2004
PMID: 16467906
Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach.
Maravita A, Spence C, Driver J., Curr. Biol. 13(13), 2003
PMID: 12842033
Maravita A, Spence C, Driver J., Curr. Biol. 13(13), 2003
PMID: 12842033
Disentangling the External Reference Frames Relevant to Tactile Localization.
Heed T, Backhaus J, Roder B, Badde S., PLoS ONE 11(7), 2016
PMID: 27391805
Heed T, Backhaus J, Roder B, Badde S., PLoS ONE 11(7), 2016
PMID: 27391805
Towards explaining spatial touch perception: Weighted integration of multiple location codes.
Badde S, Heed T., Cogn Neuropsychol 33(1-2), 2016
PMID: 27327353
Badde S, Heed T., Cogn Neuropsychol 33(1-2), 2016
PMID: 27327353
Tactile remapping beyond space.
Azanon E, Camacho K, Soto-Faraco S., Eur. J. Neurosci. 31(10), 2010
PMID: 20584190
Azanon E, Camacho K, Soto-Faraco S., Eur. J. Neurosci. 31(10), 2010
PMID: 20584190
Confusing the mind by crossing the hands
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2002
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2002
Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing.
Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S., Nat. Neurosci. 4(7), 2001
PMID: 11426234
Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S., Nat. Neurosci. 4(7), 2001
PMID: 11426234
The orienting of attention during eye and hand movements: ERP evidence for similar frame of reference but different spatially specific modulations of tactile processing.
Gherri E, Forster B., Biol Psychol 91(2), 2012
PMID: 22750676
Gherri E, Forster B., Biol Psychol 91(2), 2012
PMID: 22750676
Crossing the hands disrupts tactile spatial attention but not motor attention: evidence from event-related potentials.
Gherri E, Forster B., Neuropsychologia 50(9), 2012
PMID: 22683449
Gherri E, Forster B., Neuropsychologia 50(9), 2012
PMID: 22683449
Movement Induces the Use of External Spatial Coordinates for Tactile Localization in Congenitally Blind Humans
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2015
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2015
Predicting future sensorimotor states influences current temporal decision making.
Hermosillo R, Ritterband-Rosenbaum A, van Donkelaar P., J. Neurosci. 31(27), 2011
PMID: 21734293
Hermosillo R, Ritterband-Rosenbaum A, van Donkelaar P., J. Neurosci. 31(27), 2011
PMID: 21734293
Gaze-dependent spatial updating of tactile targets in a localization task.
Mueller S, Fiehler K., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24575060
Mueller S, Fiehler K., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24575060
Effector movement triggers gaze-dependent spatial coding of tactile and proprioceptive-tactile reach targets.
Mueller S, Fiehler K., Neuropsychologia 62(), 2014
PMID: 25084225
Mueller S, Fiehler K., Neuropsychologia 62(), 2014
PMID: 25084225
Reference frames for coding touch location depend on the task.
Pritchett LM, Carnevale MJ, Harris LR., Exp Brain Res 222(4), 2012
PMID: 22941315
Pritchett LM, Carnevale MJ, Harris LR., Exp Brain Res 222(4), 2012
PMID: 22941315
Dynamic tuning of tactile localization to body posture.
Azanon E, Stenner MP, Cardini F, Haggard P., Curr. Biol. 25(4), 2015
PMID: 25660544
Azanon E, Stenner MP, Cardini F, Haggard P., Curr. Biol. 25(4), 2015
PMID: 25660544
Early visual deprivation alters multisensory processing in peripersonal space.
Collignon O, Charbonneau G, Lassonde M, Lepore F., Neuropsychologia 47(14), 2009
PMID: 19666035
Collignon O, Charbonneau G, Lassonde M, Lepore F., Neuropsychologia 47(14), 2009
PMID: 19666035
Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind.
Roder B, Rosler F, Spence C., Curr. Biol. 14(2), 2004
PMID: 14738733
Roder B, Rosler F, Spence C., Curr. Biol. 14(2), 2004
PMID: 14738733
Spatial coordinate systems for tactile spatial attention depend on developmental vision: evidence from event-related potentials in sighted and congenitally blind adult humans.
Roder B, Focker J, Hotting K, Spence C., Eur. J. Neurosci. 28(3), 2008
PMID: 18702719
Roder B, Focker J, Hotting K, Spence C., Eur. J. Neurosci. 28(3), 2008
PMID: 18702719
Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision.
Heed T, Roder B., Cognition 132(1), 2014
PMID: 24727423
Heed T, Roder B., Cognition 132(1), 2014
PMID: 24727423
How visual experience impacts the internal and external spatial mapping of sensorimotor functions.
Crollen V, Albouy G, Lepore F, Collignon O., Sci Rep 7(1), 2017
PMID: 28432316
Crollen V, Albouy G, Lepore F, Collignon O., Sci Rep 7(1), 2017
PMID: 28432316
Space and time in the sighted and blind.
Bottini R, Crepaldi D, Casasanto D, Crollen V, Collignon O., Cognition 141(), 2015
PMID: 25935747
Bottini R, Crepaldi D, Casasanto D, Crollen V, Collignon O., Cognition 141(), 2015
PMID: 25935747
Response requirements modulate tactile spatial congruency effects.
Gallace A, Soto-Faraco S, Dalton P, Kreukniet B, Spence C., Exp Brain Res 191(2), 2008
PMID: 18709500
Gallace A, Soto-Faraco S, Dalton P, Kreukniet B, Spence C., Exp Brain Res 191(2), 2008
PMID: 18709500
Tactile selective attention and body posture: assessing the multisensory contributions of vision and proprioception.
Soto-Faraco S, Ronald A, Spence C., Percept Psychophys 66(7), 2004
PMID: 15751467
Soto-Faraco S, Ronald A, Spence C., Percept Psychophys 66(7), 2004
PMID: 15751467
Self-referent and movement cues in coding spatial location by blind and sighted children.
Millar S., Perception 10(3), 1981
PMID: 7329748
Millar S., Perception 10(3), 1981
PMID: 7329748
Event-related potential evidence for the use of external coordinates in the preparation of tactile attention by the early blind.
Eardley AF, van Velzen J., Eur. J. Neurosci. 33(10), 2011
PMID: 21545656
Eardley AF, van Velzen J., Eur. J. Neurosci. 33(10), 2011
PMID: 21545656
Interactions of different body parts in peripersonal space: how vision of the foot influences tactile perception at the hand.
Schicke T, Bauer F, Roder B., Exp Brain Res 192(4), 2008
PMID: 18841353
Schicke T, Bauer F, Roder B., Exp Brain Res 192(4), 2008
PMID: 18841353
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2009
Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models.
Jaeger TF., J Mem Lang 59(4), 2008
PMID: 19884961
Jaeger TF., J Mem Lang 59(4), 2008
PMID: 19884961
Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution.
Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MH, White JS., Trends Ecol. Evol. (Amst.) 24(3), 2009
PMID: 19185386
Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MH, White JS., Trends Ecol. Evol. (Amst.) 24(3), 2009
PMID: 19185386
Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal.
Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ., J Mem Lang 68(3), 2013
PMID: 24403724
Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ., J Mem Lang 68(3), 2013
PMID: 24403724
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Reach Trajectories Characterize Tactile Localization for Sensorimotor Decision Making.
Brandes J, Heed T., J. Neurosci. 35(40), 2015
PMID: 26446218
Brandes J, Heed T., J. Neurosci. 35(40), 2015
PMID: 26446218
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2000
A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 1979
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 1979
Common anatomical and external coding for hands and feet in tactile attention: evidence from event-related potentials.
Heed T, Roder B., J Cogn Neurosci 22(1), 2010
PMID: 19199399
Heed T, Roder B., J Cogn Neurosci 22(1), 2010
PMID: 19199399
Parietal oscillations code nonvisual reach targets relative to gaze and body.
Buchholz VN, Jensen O, Medendorp WP., J. Neurosci. 33(8), 2013
PMID: 23426676
Buchholz VN, Jensen O, Medendorp WP., J. Neurosci. 33(8), 2013
PMID: 23426676
Multiple reference frames in cortical oscillatory activity during tactile remapping for saccades.
Buchholz VN, Jensen O, Medendorp WP., J. Neurosci. 31(46), 2011
PMID: 22090512
Buchholz VN, Jensen O, Medendorp WP., J. Neurosci. 31(46), 2011
PMID: 22090512
Oscillatory activity reflects differential use of spatial reference frames by sighted and blind individuals in tactile attention.
Schubert JT, Buchholz VN, Focker J, Engel AK, Roder B, Heed T., Neuroimage 117(), 2015
PMID: 26032885
Schubert JT, Buchholz VN, Focker J, Engel AK, Roder B, Heed T., Neuroimage 117(), 2015
PMID: 26032885
Improved selective and divided spatial attention in early blind subjects.
Collignon O, Renier L, Bruyer R, Tranduy D, Veraart C., Brain Res. 1075(1), 2006
PMID: 16460716
Collignon O, Renier L, Bruyer R, Tranduy D, Veraart C., Brain Res. 1075(1), 2006
PMID: 16460716
Altered auditory-tactile interactions in congenitally blind humans: an event-related potential study.
Hotting K, Rosler F, Roder B., Exp Brain Res 159(3), 2004
PMID: 15241575
Hotting K, Rosler F, Roder B., Exp Brain Res 159(3), 2004
PMID: 15241575
Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans.
Roder B, Teder-Salejarvi W, Sterr A, Rosler F, Hillyard SA, Neville HJ., Nature 400(6740), 1999
PMID: 10408442
Roder B, Teder-Salejarvi W, Sterr A, Rosler F, Hillyard SA, Neville HJ., Nature 400(6740), 1999
PMID: 10408442
Altered tactile spatial attention in the early blind.
Forster B, Eardley AF, Eimer M., Brain Res. 1131(1), 2006
PMID: 17173872
Forster B, Eardley AF, Eimer M., Brain Res. 1131(1), 2006
PMID: 17173872
Noninformative vision improves haptic spatial perception.
Newport R, Rabb B, Jackson SR., Curr. Biol. 12(19), 2002
PMID: 12361568
Newport R, Rabb B, Jackson SR., Curr. Biol. 12(19), 2002
PMID: 12361568
Alleviating the 'crossed-hands' deficit by seeing uncrossed rubber hands.
Azanon E, Soto-Faraco S., Exp Brain Res 182(4), 2007
PMID: 17643239
Azanon E, Soto-Faraco S., Exp Brain Res 182(4), 2007
PMID: 17643239
Response demands and blindfolding in the crossed-hands deficit: an exploration of reference frame conflict
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2013
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2013
Visual capture of touch: out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves.
Pavani F, Spence C, Driver J., Psychol Sci 11(5), 2000
PMID: 11228904
Pavani F, Spence C, Driver J., Psychol Sci 11(5), 2000
PMID: 11228904
Externes Material:
Originaltext
Export
Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen
Web of Science
Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®Quellen
PMID: 29228023
PubMed | Europe PMC
Suchen in