Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use
Collins T, Schicke T, Röder B (2008)
Cognition 109(3): 363-371.
Zeitschriftenaufsatz
| Veröffentlicht | Englisch
Download
Schicke et al 2008 Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use.pdf
Autor*in
Collins, Thérèse;
Schicke, TobiasUniBi ;
Röder, Brigitte
Erscheinungsjahr
2008
Zeitschriftentitel
Cognition
Band
109
Ausgabe
3
Seite(n)
363-371
Urheberrecht / Lizenzen
ISSN
0010-0277
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2908171
Zitieren
Collins T, Schicke T, Röder B. Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition. 2008;109(3):363-371.
Collins, T., Schicke, T., & Röder, B. (2008). Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition, 109(3), 363-371. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.001
Collins, Thérèse, Schicke, Tobias, and Röder, Brigitte. 2008. “Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use”. Cognition 109 (3): 363-371.
Collins, T., Schicke, T., and Röder, B. (2008). Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition 109, 363-371.
Collins, T., Schicke, T., & Röder, B., 2008. Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition, 109(3), p 363-371.
T. Collins, T. Schicke, and B. Röder, “Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use”, Cognition, vol. 109, 2008, pp. 363-371.
Collins, T., Schicke, T., Röder, B.: Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition. 109, 363-371 (2008).
Collins, Thérèse, Schicke, Tobias, and Röder, Brigitte. “Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use”. Cognition 109.3 (2008): 363-371.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Creative Commons Namensnennung 3.0 Unported (CC BY 3.0):
Volltext(e)
Name
Schicke et al 2008 Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use.pdf
Access Level
UniBi Only
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-06T09:18:43Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
8feb4ca6df0e8b2046e6d9426feb35c6
Link(s) zu Volltext(en)
Access Level
Closed Access
Daten bereitgestellt von European Bioinformatics Institute (EBI)
21 Zitationen in Europe PMC
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Visuomotor and motorvisual priming with different types of set-level congruency: evidence in support of ideomotor theory, and the planning and control model (PCM).
Thomaschke R, Miall RC, Rueß M, Mehta PR, Hopkins B., Psychol Res 82(6), 2018
PMID: 28756514
Thomaschke R, Miall RC, Rueß M, Mehta PR, Hopkins B., Psychol Res 82(6), 2018
PMID: 28756514
Eye-hand coordination during visuomotor adaptation: effects of hemispace and joint coordination.
Rand MK, Rentsch S., Exp Brain Res 235(12), 2017
PMID: 28900673
Rand MK, Rentsch S., Exp Brain Res 235(12), 2017
PMID: 28900673
Effects of Reliability and Global Context on Explicit and Implicit Measures of Sensed Hand Position in Cursor-Control Tasks.
Rand MK, Heuer H., Front Psychol 6(), 2015
PMID: 26793162
Rand MK, Heuer H., Front Psychol 6(), 2015
PMID: 26793162
Too much anticipation? Large anticipatory adjustments of grasping movements to minimal object manipulations.
Herbort O., Hum Mov Sci 42(), 2015
PMID: 26004123
Herbort O., Hum Mov Sci 42(), 2015
PMID: 26004123
What does the forward model of an expert hand-tool motor program code?: Comment on "Action semantics: A unifying conceptual framework for the selective use of multimodal and modality-specific object knowledge" by van Elk, van Schie, and Bekkering.
Soliman T, Glenberg AM., Phys Life Rev 11(2), 2014
PMID: 24388275
Soliman T, Glenberg AM., Phys Life Rev 11(2), 2014
PMID: 24388275
The contribution of cognitive, kinematic, and dynamic factors to anticipatory grasp selection.
Herbort O, Butz MV, Kunde W., Exp Brain Res 232(6), 2014
PMID: 24534913
Herbort O, Butz MV, Kunde W., Exp Brain Res 232(6), 2014
PMID: 24534913
Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision.
Heed T, Röder B., Cognition 132(1), 2014
PMID: 24727423
Heed T, Röder B., Cognition 132(1), 2014
PMID: 24727423
Segment interdependency and gaze anchoring during manual two-segment sequences.
Rand MK., Exp Brain Res 232(9), 2014
PMID: 24770857
Rand MK., Exp Brain Res 232(9), 2014
PMID: 24770857
In manually-assisted search, perception supervises rather than directs action.
Solman GJ, Wu N, Cheyne JA, Smilek D., Exp Psychol 60(4), 2013
PMID: 23422655
Solman GJ, Wu N, Cheyne JA, Smilek D., Exp Psychol 60(4), 2013
PMID: 23422655
Allocation of attention for dissociated visual and motor goals.
Song JH, Bédard P., Exp Brain Res 226(2), 2013
PMID: 23417647
Song JH, Bédard P., Exp Brain Res 226(2), 2013
PMID: 23417647
Vision and proprioception in action monitoring by young and older adults.
Rand MK, Wang L, Müsseler J, Heuer H., Neurobiol Aging 34(7), 2013
PMID: 23433708
Rand MK, Wang L, Müsseler J, Heuer H., Neurobiol Aging 34(7), 2013
PMID: 23433708
Moving further moves things further away in visual perception: position-based movement planning affects distance judgments.
Kirsch W, Kunde W., Exp Brain Res 226(3), 2013
PMID: 23455728
Kirsch W, Kunde W., Exp Brain Res 226(3), 2013
PMID: 23455728
Movement paths in operating hand-held tools: tests of distal-shift hypotheses.
Sülzenbrück S, Heuer H., J Neurophysiol 109(11), 2013
PMID: 23486203
Sülzenbrück S, Heuer H., J Neurophysiol 109(11), 2013
PMID: 23486203
Implicit and explicit representations of hand position in tool use.
Rand MK, Heuer H., PLoS One 8(7), 2013
PMID: 23894307
Rand MK, Heuer H., PLoS One 8(7), 2013
PMID: 23894307
The role of cue-response mapping in motorvisual impairment and facilitation: evidence for different roles of action planning and action control in motorvisual dual-task priming.
Thomaschke R, Hopkins B, Miall RC., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38(2), 2012
PMID: 21806310
Thomaschke R, Hopkins B, Miall RC., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38(2), 2012
PMID: 21806310
The planning and control model (PCM) of motorvisual priming: reconciling motorvisual impairment and facilitation effects.
Thomaschke R, Hopkins B, Miall RC., Psychol Rev 119(2), 2012
PMID: 22369178
Thomaschke R, Hopkins B, Miall RC., Psychol Rev 119(2), 2012
PMID: 22369178
Investigating ideomotor cognition with motorvisual priming paradigms: key findings, methodological challenges, and future directions.
Thomaschke R., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23189067
Thomaschke R., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23189067
Using a stick does not necessarily alter judged distances or reachability.
de Grave DD, Brenner E, Smeets JB., PLoS One 6(2), 2011
PMID: 21390215
de Grave DD, Brenner E, Smeets JB., PLoS One 6(2), 2011
PMID: 21390215
Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping.
Baldauf D, Deubel H., Vision Res 50(11), 2010
PMID: 20219518
Baldauf D, Deubel H., Vision Res 50(11), 2010
PMID: 20219518
Visual target selection and motor planning define attentional enhancement at perceptual processing stages.
Collins T, Heed T, Röder B., Front Hum Neurosci 4(), 2010
PMID: 20224662
Collins T, Heed T, Röder B., Front Hum Neurosci 4(), 2010
PMID: 20224662
The cognitive neuroscience of prehension: recent developments.
Grafton ST., Exp Brain Res 204(4), 2010
PMID: 20532487
Grafton ST., Exp Brain Res 204(4), 2010
PMID: 20532487
31 References
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements.
Baldauf D, Wolf M, Deubel H., Vision Res. 46(26), 2006
PMID: 17034829
Baldauf D, Wolf M, Deubel H., Vision Res. 46(26), 2006
PMID: 17034829
Properties of attentional selection during the preparation of sequential saccades.
Baldauf D, Deubel H., Exp Brain Res 184(3), 2007
PMID: 17846754
Baldauf D, Deubel H., Exp Brain Res 184(3), 2007
PMID: 17846754
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Task combination and selective intake of information.
Broadbent DE., Acta Psychol (Amst) 50(3), 1982
PMID: 7124432
Broadbent DE., Acta Psychol (Amst) 50(3), 1982
PMID: 7124432
Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism.
Deubel H, Schneider WX., Vision Res. 36(12), 1996
PMID: 8759451
Deubel H, Schneider WX., Vision Res. 36(12), 1996
PMID: 8759451
Attentional selection in sequential manual movements, movements around an obstacle and in grasping
Deubel, 2004
Deubel, 2004
Selective dorsal and ventral processing: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism in reaching and perception
Deubel, Visual Cognition 5(), 1998
Deubel, Visual Cognition 5(), 1998
Attentional selection during preparation of eye movements.
Dore-Mazars K, Pouget P, Beauvillain C., Psychol Res 69(1-2), 2004
PMID: 15138819
Dore-Mazars K, Pouget P, Beauvillain C., Psychol Res 69(1-2), 2004
PMID: 15138819
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Movement and visual attention: The spotlight metaphor breaks down
Driver, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15(), 1989
Driver, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15(), 1989
The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements.
Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME., Science 255(5040), 1992
PMID: 1553535
Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME., Science 255(5040), 1992
PMID: 1553535
Parallel allocation of attention prior to the execution of saccade sequences.
Godijn R, Theeuwes J., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29(5), 2003
PMID: 14585012
Godijn R, Theeuwes J., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29(5), 2003
PMID: 14585012
Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior.
Graziano MS, Cooke DF., Neuropsychologia 44(6), 2005
PMID: 16277998
Graziano MS, Cooke DF., Neuropsychologia 44(6), 2005
PMID: 16277998
Further evidence for the division of attention among noncontiguous locations
Hahn, Visual Cognition 5(), 1998
Hahn, Visual Cognition 5(), 1998
The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space.
Holmes NP, Spence C., Cogn Process 5(2), 2004
PMID: 16467906
Holmes NP, Spence C., Cogn Process 5(2), 2004
PMID: 16467906
Extending or projecting peripersonal space with tools? Multisensory interactions highlight only the distal and proximal ends of tools.
Holmes NP, Calvert GA, Spence C., Neurosci. Lett. 372(1-2), 2004
PMID: 15531089
Holmes NP, Calvert GA, Spence C., Neurosci. Lett. 372(1-2), 2004
PMID: 15531089
Tool-use: capturing multisensory spatial attention or extending multisensory peripersonal space?
Holmes NP, Sanabria D, Calvert GA, Spence C., Cortex 43(3), 2007
PMID: 17533769
Holmes NP, Sanabria D, Calvert GA, Spence C., Cortex 43(3), 2007
PMID: 17533769
Direct neurophysiological evidence for spatial suppression surrounding the focus of attention in vision.
Hopf JM, Boehler CN, Luck SJ, Tsotsos JK, Heinze HJ, Schoenfeld MA., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103(4), 2006
PMID: 16410356
Hopf JM, Boehler CN, Luck SJ, Tsotsos JK, Heinze HJ, Schoenfeld MA., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103(4), 2006
PMID: 16410356
The statistics of natural hand movements.
Ingram JN, Kording KP, Howard IS, Wolpert DM., Exp Brain Res 188(2), 2008
PMID: 18369608
Ingram JN, Kording KP, Howard IS, Wolpert DM., Exp Brain Res 188(2), 2008
PMID: 18369608
Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones.
Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y., Neuroreport 7(14), 1996
PMID: 8951846
Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y., Neuroreport 7(14), 1996
PMID: 8951846
Splitting the beam: distribution of attention over noncontiguous regions of the visual field
Kramer, Psychological Science 6(), 1995
Kramer, Psychological Science 6(), 1995
Spatial extent of attention to letters and words
LaBerge, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9(), 1983
LaBerge, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9(), 1983
Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans.
Maravita A, Spence C, Kennett S, Driver J., Cognition 83(2), 2002
PMID: 11869727
Maravita A, Spence C, Kennett S, Driver J., Cognition 83(2), 2002
PMID: 11869727
Sustained division of the attentional spotlight.
Muller MM, Malinowski P, Gruber T, Hillyard SA., Nature 424(6946), 2003
PMID: 12867981
Muller MM, Malinowski P, Gruber T, Hillyard SA., Nature 424(6946), 2003
PMID: 12867981
Attentional selection during preparation of prehension movements
Schiegg, Visual Cognition 10(), 2003
Schiegg, Visual Cognition 10(), 2003
Multisensory contributions to the 3-D representation of visuotactile peripersonal space in humans: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task.
Spence C, Pavani F, Maravita A, Holmes N., J. Physiol. Paris 98(1-3), 2004
PMID: 15477031
Spence C, Pavani F, Maravita A, Holmes N., J. Physiol. Paris 98(1-3), 2004
PMID: 15477031
The blinking spotlight of attention.
VanRullen R, Carlson T, Cavanagh P., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104(49), 2007
PMID: 18042716
VanRullen R, Carlson T, Cavanagh P., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104(49), 2007
PMID: 18042716
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Export
Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen
Web of Science
Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®Quellen
PMID: 19012884
PubMed | Europe PMC
Suchen in