Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision

Heed T, Röder B (2014)
Cognition 132(1): 1-15.

Zeitschriftenaufsatz | Veröffentlicht | Englisch
 
Download
Campus/VPN Heed et al. 2014 Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates.pdf
Autor*in
Heed, TobiasUniBi ; Röder, Brigitte
Erscheinungsjahr
2014
Zeitschriftentitel
Cognition
Band
132
Ausgabe
1
Seite(n)
1-15
ISSN
0010-0277
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2907388

Zitieren

Heed T, Röder B. Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision. Cognition. 2014;132(1):1-15.
Heed, T., & Röder, B. (2014). Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision. Cognition, 132(1), 1-15. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.005
Heed, Tobias, and Röder, Brigitte. 2014. “Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision”. Cognition 132 (1): 1-15.
Heed, T., and Röder, B. (2014). Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision. Cognition 132, 1-15.
Heed, T., & Röder, B., 2014. Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision. Cognition, 132(1), p 1-15.
T. Heed and B. Röder, “Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision”, Cognition, vol. 132, 2014, pp. 1-15.
Heed, T., Röder, B.: Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision. Cognition. 132, 1-15 (2014).
Heed, Tobias, and Röder, Brigitte. “Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates independent of developmental vision”. Cognition 132.1 (2014): 1-15.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Volltext(e)
Name
Heed et al. 2014 Motor coordination uses external spatial coordinates.pdf
Access Level
Campus/VPN UniBi Only
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-06T09:18:41Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
f02af91ad1f2a7c8218ea84eea0a8c02


Link(s) zu Volltext(en)
Access Level
Restricted Closed Access

7 Zitationen in Europe PMC

Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.

Anticipatory action planning in blind and sighted individuals.
Cavallo A, Ansuini C, Gori M, Tinti C, Tonelli A, Becchio C., Sci Rep 7(), 2017
PMID: 28304373
How visual experience impacts the internal and external spatial mapping of sensorimotor functions.
Crollen V, Albouy G, Lepore F, Collignon O., Sci Rep 7(1), 2017
PMID: 28432316
Visual Experience Shapes the Neural Networks Remapping Touch into External Space.
Crollen V, Lazzouni L, Rezk M, Bellemare A, Lepore F, Collignon O., J Neurosci 37(42), 2017
PMID: 28947578
Space and time in the sighted and blind.
Bottini R, Crepaldi D, Casasanto D, Crollen V, Collignon O., Cognition 141(), 2015
PMID: 25935747
Oscillatory activity reflects differential use of spatial reference frames by sighted and blind individuals in tactile attention.
Schubert JT, Buchholz VN, Föcker J, Engel AK, Röder B, Heed T., Neuroimage 117(), 2015
PMID: 26032885

58 References

Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.

Tactile remapping beyond space.
Azanon E, Camacho K, Soto-Faraco S., Eur. J. Neurosci. 31(10), 2010
PMID: 20584190
Multiple spatial representations determine touch localization on the fingers
Badde, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (), 2013
Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping.
Baldauf D, Deubel H., Vision Res. 50(11), 2010
PMID: 20219518
Preferential coupling between voluntary movements of ipsilateral limbs.
Baldissera F, Cavallari P, Civaschi P., Neurosci. Lett. 34(1), 1982
PMID: 7162702
Random effects structure for testing interactions in linear mixed-effects models
Barr, Frontiers in Psychology 4(), 2013
Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal
Barr, Journal of Memory and Language 68(3), 2013

AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates.
Batista AP, Buneo CA, Snyder LH, Andersen RA., Science 285(5425), 1999
PMID: 10398603
Polymodal motion processing in posterior parietal and premotor cortex: a human fMRI study strongly implies equivalencies between humans and monkeys.
Bremmer F, Schlack A, Shah NJ, Zafiris O, Kubischik M, Hoffmann K, Zilles K, Fink GR., Neuron 29(1), 2001
PMID: 11182099
Eye-movement-driven changes in the perception of auditory space
Collins, Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics 72(3), 2010
Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use.
Collins T, Schicke T, Roder B., Cognition 109(3), 2008
PMID: 19012884
Vestibular reafference shapes voluntary movement.
Day BL, Reynolds RF., Curr. Biol. 15(15), 2005
PMID: 16085491
Models of accuracy in repeated-measures designs
Dixon, Journal of Memory and Language 59(4), 2008
The role of target information on manual-aiming bias
Elliott, Psychological Research 58(1), 1995
The human dorsal action control system develops in the absence of vision.
Fiehler K, Burke M, Bien S, Roder B, Rosler F., Cereb. Cortex 19(1), 2008
PMID: 18448452
The SURE_REACH model for motor learning and control of a redundant arm: From modeling human behavior to applications in robotics
Herbort, 2010
The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity.
Hopp JJ, Fuchs AF., Prog. Neurobiol. 72(1), 2004
PMID: 15019175
Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination
Kelso, American Journal of Physiology – Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 246(6), 1984
Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: Critical fluctuations
Kelso, Physics Letters A 118(6), 1986
Choosing goals, not rules: deciding among rule-based action plans.
Klaes C, Westendorff S, Chakrabarti S, Gail A., Neuron 70(3), 2011
PMID: 21555078
Effects of task instructions and oscillation frequency on bimanual coordination.
Lee TD, Blandin Y, Proteau L., Psychol Res 59(2), 1996
PMID: 8810585
A psychological approach to human voluntary movement.
Mechsner F., J Mot Behav 36(4), 2004
PMID: 15695214
Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination.
Mechsner F, Kerzel D, Knoblich G, Prinz W., Nature 414(6859), 2001
PMID: 11689944
Do muscles matter for coordinated action?
Mechsner, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30(3), 2004
Remapping the remembered target location for anti-saccades in human posterior parietal cortex.
Medendorp WP, Goltz HC, Vilis T., J. Neurophysiol. 94(1), 2005
PMID: 15788514
Multisensory spatial representations in eye-centered coordinates for reaching.
Pouget A, Ducom JC, Torri J, Bavelier D., Cognition 83(1), 2002
PMID: 11814488
Stimulus-locked responses on human arm muscles reveal a rapid neural pathway linking visual input to arm motor output.
Pruszynski JA, King GL, Boisse L, Scott SH, Flanagan JR, Munoz DP., Eur. J. Neurosci. 32(6), 2010
PMID: 20726884

AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Spatial remapping in the audio-tactile ventriloquism effect: A TMS investigation on the role of the ventral intraparietal area
Renzi, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (), 2013
Spatial updating depends on gaze direction even after loss of vision.
Reuschel J, Rosler F, Henriques DY, Fiehler K., J. Neurosci. 32(7), 2012
PMID: 22396416
Spatial and muscular dependencies in bimanual coordination
Riek, Journal of Human Movement Studies 23(), 1992
Hierarchical organisation of neuro-anatomical constraints in interlimb coordination
Riek, Human Movement Science 24(5–6), 2005
Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention
Rizzolatti, Neuropsychologia 25(1), 1987
Developmental vision determines the reference frame for the multisensory control of action.
Roder B, Kusmierek A, Spence C, Schicke T., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104(11), 2007
PMID: 17360596
Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind.
Roder B, Rosler F, Spence C., Curr. Biol. 14(2), 2004
PMID: 14738733
Influence of visual information on the auditory median plane of the head.
Schicke T, Demuth L, Roder B., Neuroreport 13(13), 2002
PMID: 12352615
Parietal cortex codes for egocentric space beyond the field of view.
Schindler A, Bartels A., Curr. Biol. 23(2), 2012
PMID: 23260468
Confusing the mind by crossing the hands.
Shore DI, Spry E, Spence C., Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 14(1), 2002
PMID: 12063139

AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Exploring interlimb constraints during bimanual graphic performance: effects of muscle grouping and direction.
Swinnen SP, Jardin K, Verschueren S, Meulenbroek R, Franz L, Dounskaia N, Walter CB., Behav. Brain Res. 90(1), 1998
PMID: 9520215
Interaction of directional, neuromuscular and egocentric constraints on the stability of preferred bimanual coordination patterns.
Temprado JJ, Swinnen SP, Carson RG, Tourment A, Laurent M., Hum Mov Sci 22(3), 2003
PMID: 12967762
Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense?
Thinus-Blanc C, Gaunet F., Psychol Bull 121(1), 1997
PMID: 9064698

Wickham, 2009
Are arm trajectories planned in kinematic or dynamic coordinates? An adaptation study.
Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI., Exp Brain Res 103(3), 1995
PMID: 7789452
Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing.
Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S., Nat. Neurosci. 4(7), 2001
PMID: 11426234
Export

Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen

Open Data PUB

Web of Science

Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®
Quellen

PMID: 24727423
PubMed | Europe PMC

Suchen in

Google Scholar