Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search
Horstmann G, Herwig A, Becker SI (2016)
Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1152.
Zeitschriftenaufsatz
| Veröffentlicht | Englisch
Download
fpsyg-07-01152.horstmann.pdf
2.21 MB
Autor*in
Einrichtung
Abstract / Bemerkung
Some targets in visual search are more difficult to find than others. In particular, a target that is similar to the distractors is more difficult to find than a target that is dissimilar to the distractors. Efficiency differences between easy and difficult searches are manifest not only in target-present trials but also in target-absent trials. In fact, even physically identical displays are searched through with different efficiency depending on the searched-for target. Here, we monitored eye movements in search for a target similar to the distractors (difficult search) versus a target dissimilar to the distractors (easy search). We aimed to examine three hypotheses concerning the causes of differential search efficiencies in target-absent trials: (a) distractor dwelling (b) distractor skipping, and (c) distractor revisiting. Reaction times increased with target similarity which is consistent with existing theories and replicates earlier results. Eye movement data indicated guidance in target trials, even though search was very slow. Dwelling, skipping, and revisiting contributed to low search efficiency in difficult search, with dwelling being the strongest factor. It is argued that differences in dwell time account for a large amount of total search time differences.
Erscheinungsjahr
2016
Zeitschriftentitel
Frontiers in Psychology
Band
7
Art.-Nr.
1152
ISSN
1664-1078
Finanzierungs-Informationen
Open-Access-Publikationskosten wurden durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft und die Universität Bielefeld gefördert.
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2905221
Zitieren
Horstmann G, Herwig A, Becker SI. Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7: 1152.
Horstmann, G., Herwig, A., & Becker, S. I. (2016). Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1152. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01152
Horstmann, Gernot, Herwig, Arvid, and Becker, Stefanie I. 2016. “Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search”. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1152.
Horstmann, G., Herwig, A., and Becker, S. I. (2016). Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search. Frontiers in Psychology 7:1152.
Horstmann, G., Herwig, A., & Becker, S.I., 2016. Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search. Frontiers in Psychology, 7: 1152.
G. Horstmann, A. Herwig, and S.I. Becker, “Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search”, Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 7, 2016, : 1152.
Horstmann, G., Herwig, A., Becker, S.I.: Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search. Frontiers in Psychology. 7, : 1152 (2016).
Horstmann, Gernot, Herwig, Arvid, and Becker, Stefanie I. “Distractor Dwelling, Skipping, and Revisiting Determine Target Absent Performance in Difficult Visual Search”. Frontiers in Psychology 7 (2016): 1152.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Copyright Statement:
Dieses Objekt ist durch das Urheberrecht und/oder verwandte Schutzrechte geschützt. [...]
Volltext(e)
Name
fpsyg-07-01152.horstmann.pdf
2.21 MB
Access Level
Open Access
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-06T09:18:39Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
2cdcac67e7f9a91c1cd5682f5303d2ad
Link(s) zu Volltext(en)
Access Level
Closed Access
Daten bereitgestellt von European Bioinformatics Institute (EBI)
1 Zitation in Europe PMC
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Developmental effects of stimulus gender and the social context in which it appears on threat detection.
Horovitz O, Lindenfeld I, Melamed M, Shechner T., Br J Dev Psychol 36(3), 2018
PMID: 29266313
Horovitz O, Lindenfeld I, Melamed M, Shechner T., Br J Dev Psychol 36(3), 2018
PMID: 29266313
32 References
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Visual similarity effects in categorical search.
Alexander RG, Zelinsky GJ., J Vis 11(8), 2011
PMID: 21757505
Alexander RG, Zelinsky GJ., J Vis 11(8), 2011
PMID: 21757505
Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture.
Bacon WF, Egeth HE., Percept Psychophys 55(5), 1994
PMID: 8008550
Bacon WF, Egeth HE., Percept Psychophys 55(5), 1994
PMID: 8008550
Determinants of dwell time in visual search: similarity or perceptual difficulty?
Becker SI., PLoS ONE 6(3), 2011
PMID: 21408139
Becker SI., PLoS ONE 6(3), 2011
PMID: 21408139
Just say no: how are visual searches terminated when there is no target present?
Chun MM, Wolfe JM., Cogn Psychol 30(1), 1996
PMID: 8635311
Chun MM, Wolfe JM., Cogn Psychol 30(1), 1996
PMID: 8635311
Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism.
Deubel H, Schneider WX., Vision Res. 36(12), 1996
PMID: 8759451
Deubel H, Schneider WX., Vision Res. 36(12), 1996
PMID: 8759451
Visual search and stimulus similarity.
Duncan J, Humphreys GW., Psychol Rev 96(3), 1989
PMID: 2756067
Duncan J, Humphreys GW., Psychol Rev 96(3), 1989
PMID: 2756067
Refixation frequency and memory mechanisms in visual search.
Gilchrist ID, Harvey M., Curr. Biol. 10(19), 2000
PMID: 11050390
Gilchrist ID, Harvey M., Curr. Biol. 10(19), 2000
PMID: 11050390
Pattern-recognition and eye-movement parameters.
Gould J.., 1967
Gould J.., 1967
Linking perception and action by structure or process? Toward an integrative perspective.
Herwig A., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 52(), 2015
PMID: 25732773
Herwig A., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 52(), 2015
PMID: 25732773
Control of fixation duration in a simple search task.
Hooge IT, Erkelens CJ., Percept Psychophys 58(7), 1996
PMID: 8920834
Hooge IT, Erkelens CJ., Percept Psychophys 58(7), 1996
PMID: 8920834
Adjustment of fixation duration in visual search.
Hooge IT, Erkelens CJ., Vision Res. 38(9), 1998
PMID: 9666997
Hooge IT, Erkelens CJ., Vision Res. 38(9), 1998
PMID: 9666997
Of toothy grins and angry snarls--open mouth displays contribute to efficiency gains in search for emotional faces.
Horstmann G, Lipp OV, Becker SI., J Vis 12(5), 2012
PMID: 22637708
Horstmann G, Lipp OV, Becker SI., J Vis 12(5), 2012
PMID: 22637708
The impending demise of the item in visual search.
Hulleman J., Olivers C.., 2016
Hulleman J., Olivers C.., 2016
Search via Recursive Rejection (SERR): a connectionist model of visual search.
Humphreys G., Müller H.., 1993
Humphreys G., Müller H.., 1993
A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention.
Itti L, Koch C., Vision Res. 40(10-12), 2000
PMID: 10788654
Itti L, Koch C., Vision Res. 40(10-12), 2000
PMID: 10788654
Top-down attention selection is fine grained.
Navalpakkam V, Itti L., J Vis 6(11), 2006
PMID: 17209728
Navalpakkam V, Itti L., J Vis 6(11), 2006
PMID: 17209728
The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli.
Ohman A, Lundqvist D, Esteves F., J Pers Soc Psychol 80(3), 2001
PMID: 11300573
Ohman A, Lundqvist D, Esteves F., J Pers Soc Psychol 80(3), 2001
PMID: 11300573
Different states in visual working memory: when it guides attention and when it does not.
Olivers CN, Peters J, Houtkamp R, Roelfsema PR., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 15(7), 2011
PMID: 21665518
Olivers CN, Peters J, Houtkamp R, Roelfsema PR., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 15(7), 2011
PMID: 21665518
Target-distractor discriminability in visual search.
Pashler H.., 1987
Pashler H.., 1987
Visual search has memory.
Peterson MS, Kramer AF, Wang RF, Irwin DE, McCarley JS., Psychol Sci 12(4), 2001
PMID: 11476094
Peterson MS, Kramer AF, Wang RF, Irwin DE, McCarley JS., Psychol Sci 12(4), 2001
PMID: 11476094
VAM: a neuro-cognitive model for visual attention control of segmentation, object recognition, and space-based motor action.
Schneider W.., 1995
Schneider W.., 1995
Attentional selection in visual perception, memory and action: a quest for cross-domain integration.
Schneider WX, Einhauser W, Horstmann G., Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 368(1628), 2013
PMID: 24018715
Schneider WX, Einhauser W, Horstmann G., Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 368(1628), 2013
PMID: 24018715
Memory-scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments.
Sternberg S., Am. Sci. 57(4), 1969
PMID: 5360276
Sternberg S., Am. Sci. 57(4), 1969
PMID: 5360276
Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection.
Theeuwes J., Acta Psychol (Amst) 135(2), 2010
PMID: 20507828
Theeuwes J., Acta Psychol (Amst) 135(2), 2010
PMID: 20507828
The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants.
Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, Marcus DJ, Westerlund A, Casey BJ, Nelson C., Psychiatry Res 168(3), 2009
PMID: 19564050
Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, Marcus DJ, Westerlund A, Casey BJ, Nelson C., Psychiatry Res 168(3), 2009
PMID: 19564050
Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search.
Wolfe JM., Psychon Bull Rev 1(2), 1994
PMID: 24203471
Wolfe JM., Psychon Bull Rev 1(2), 1994
PMID: 24203471
What do 1,000,000 trials tell us about visual search?
Wolfe J.., 1998
Wolfe J.., 1998
Guided search 4.0: current progress with a model of visual search
Wolfe J.., 2007
Wolfe J.., 2007
A theory of eye movements during target acquisition.
Zelinsky GJ., Psychol Rev 115(4), 2008
PMID: 18954205
Zelinsky GJ., Psychol Rev 115(4), 2008
PMID: 18954205
Export
Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen
Web of Science
Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®Quellen
PMID: 27574510
PubMed | Europe PMC
Suchen in