Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands

Schütz C, Schack T (2015)
Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1358.

Zeitschriftenaufsatz | Veröffentlicht | Englisch
 
Download
OA
Abstract / Bemerkung
In a sequential task, the grasp postures people select depend on their movement history. This motor hysteresis effect results from the reuse of former movement plans and reduces the cognitive cost of movement planning. Movement plans for hand trajectories not only transfer across successive trials, but also across hands. We therefore asked whether such a transfer would also be found in movement plans for hand postures. To this end, we designed a sequential, continuous posture selection task. Participants had to open a column of drawers with cylindrical knobs in ascending and descending sequences. A hand switch was required in each sequence. Hand pro/supination was analyzed directly before and after the hand switch. Results showed that hysteresis effects were present directly before, but absent directly after the hand switch. This indicates that, in the current study, movement plans for hand postures only transfer across trials, but not across hands.
Stichworte
motor planning; motor hysteresis; posture selection; transfer; motor control
Erscheinungsjahr
2015
Zeitschriftentitel
Frontiers in Psychology
Band
6
Art.-Nr.
1358
ISSN
1664-1078
Finanzierungs-Informationen
Open-Access-Publikationskosten wurden durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft und die Universität Bielefeld gefördert.
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2777493

Zitieren

Schütz C, Schack T. Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6: 1358.
Schütz, C., & Schack, T. (2015). Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1358. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01358
Schütz, Christoph, and Schack, Thomas. 2015. “Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands”. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1358.
Schütz, C., and Schack, T. (2015). Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands. Frontiers in Psychology 6:1358.
Schütz, C., & Schack, T., 2015. Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 1358.
C. Schütz and T. Schack, “Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands”, Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 6, 2015, : 1358.
Schütz, C., Schack, T.: Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands. Frontiers in Psychology. 6, : 1358 (2015).
Schütz, Christoph, and Schack, Thomas. “Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands”. Frontiers in Psychology 6 (2015): 1358.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Copyright Statement:
Dieses Objekt ist durch das Urheberrecht und/oder verwandte Schutzrechte geschützt. [...]
Volltext(e)
Access Level
OA Open Access
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-06T09:18:33Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
85ecc7d38dec11558a256f7bddbcabc3


1 Zitation in Europe PMC

Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.

Motor hysteresis in a sequential grasping and pointing task is absent in task-critical joints.
Schütz C, Weigelt M, Schack T., Exp Brain Res 235(3), 2017
PMID: 27864596

36 References

Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.

Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements.
Atkeson CG, Hollerbach JM., J. Neurosci. 5(9), 1985
PMID: 4031998
No evidence for visuomotor priming in a visually guided action task.
Cant JS, Westwood DA, Valyear KF, Goodale MA., Neuropsychologia 43(2), 2005
PMID: 15707906
Will Julia Roberts harm Nicole Kidman? Semantic priming effects during face naming.
Vitkovitch M, Potton A, Bakogianni C, Kinch L., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 59(6), 2006
PMID: 16885147
Use-dependent and error-based learning of motor behaviors.
Diedrichsen J, White O, Newman D, Lally N., J. Neurosci. 30(15), 2010
PMID: 20392938
Naming and grasping common objects: a priming study.
Garofeanu C, Kroliczak G, Goodale MA, Humphrey GK., Exp Brain Res 159(1), 2004
PMID: 15221162
Separate visual pathways for perception and action.
Goodale MA, Milner AD., Trends Neurosci. 15(1), 1992
PMID: 1374953
Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of precentral cortex.
Graziano MS, Taylor CS, Moore T., Neuron 34(5), 2002
PMID: 12062029
Arm movements evoked by electrical stimulation in the motor cortex of monkeys.
Graziano MS, Aflalo TN, Cooke DF., J. Neurophysiol. 94(6), 2005
PMID: 16120657
Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning.
Harris CM, Wolpert DM., Nature 394(6695), 1998
PMID: 9723616
An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements.
Hogan N., J. Neurosci. 4(11), 1984
PMID: 6502203
Goal-related planning constraints in bimanual grasping and placing of objects.
Hughes CM, Franz EA., Exp Brain Res 188(4), 2008
PMID: 18443769
Combined effects of planning and execution constraints on bimanual task performance.
Janssen L, Beuting M, Meulenbroek R, Steenbergen B., Exp Brain Res 192(1), 2008
PMID: 18751972
Behavioral evidence for left-hemisphere specialization of motor planning.
Janssen L, Meulenbroek RG, Steenbergen B., Exp Brain Res 209(1), 2010
PMID: 21184219
Computational motor control and human factors: modeling movements in real and possible environments.
Jax SA, Rosenbaum DA, Vaughan J, Meulenbroek RG., Hum Factors 45(1), 2003
PMID: 12916579
Multifunctionality and switching in the coordination dynamics of reaching and grasping
Kelso J., Buchanan J., Murata T.., 1994
Mersenne twister: a 623-dimensionally equidistributed uniform pseudo-random number generator
Matsumoto M., Nishimura T.., 1998
Limb-segment selection in drawing behaviour.
Meulenbroek RG, Rosenbaum DA, Thomassen AJ, Schomaker LR., Q J Exp Psychol A 46(2), 1993
PMID: 8316638
The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.
Oldfield RC., Neuropsychologia 9(1), 1971
PMID: 5146491
The problem of serial order in behavior: Lashley's legacy.
Rosenbaum DA, Cohen RG, Jax SA, Weiss DJ, van der Wel R., Hum Mov Sci 26(4), 2007
PMID: 17698232
Cognition, action, and object manipulation.
Rosenbaum DA, Chapman KM, Weigelt M, Weiss DJ, van der Wel R., Psychol Bull 138(5), 2012
PMID: 22448912
Knowledge Model for Selecting and Producing Reaching Movements.
Rosenbaum DA, Engelbrecht SE, Bushe MM, Loukopoulos LD., J Mot Behav 25(3), 1993
PMID: 12581991
A model for reaching control.
Rosenbaum DA, Engelbrecht SE, Bushe MM, Loukopoulos LD., Acta Psychol (Amst) 82(1-3), 1993
PMID: 8475768
Influence of mechanical load on sequential effects.
Schutz C, Schack T., Exp Brain Res 228(4), 2013
PMID: 23727830
Motor control strategies in a continuous task space.
Schutz C, Weigelt M, Odekerken D, Klein-Soetebier T, Schack T., Motor Control 15(3), 2011
PMID: 21878687
Precision hypothesis and the end-state comfort effect.
Short MW, Cauraugh JH., Acta Psychol (Amst) 100(3), 1999
PMID: 9894689
The functional role of working memory in the (re-)planning and execution of grasping movements.
Spiegel MA, Koester D, Schack T., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 39(5), 2013
PMID: 23339349
Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: evidence for abstract spatiotemporal forms in human motor control.
van der Wel RP, Fleckenstein RM, Jax SA, Rosenbaum DA., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33(5), 2007
PMID: 17924811
End-state comfort in bimanual object manipulation.
Weigelt M, Kunde W, Prinz W., Exp Psychol 53(2), 2006
PMID: 16909939
Moving and memorizing: motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall.
Weigelt M, Rosenbaum DA, Huelshorst S, Schack T., Acta Psychol (Amst) 132(1), 2009
PMID: 19591968
Hysteresis effects in a motor task with cotton-top tamarins (Sanguinus oedipus).
Weiss DJ, Wark J., J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 35(3), 2009
PMID: 19594287
Export

Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen

Open Data PUB

Web of Science

Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®
Quellen

PMID: 26441734
PubMed | Europe PMC

Suchen in

Google Scholar