Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance
van Dam L, Ernst MO (2013)
PLOS ONE 8(10): e78757.
Zeitschriftenaufsatz
| Veröffentlicht | Englisch
Download
Autor*in
Einrichtung
Abstract / Bemerkung
Random errors are omnipresent in sensorimotor tasks due to perceptual and motor noise. The question is, are humans aware of their random errors on an instance-by-instance basis? The appealing answer would be ‘no’ because it seems intuitive that humans would otherwise immediately correct for the errors online, thereby increasing sensorimotor precision. However, here we show the opposite. Participants pointed to visual targets with varying degree of feedback. After movement completion participants indicated whether they believed they landed left or right of target. Surprisingly, participants' left/right-discriminability was well above chance, even without visual feedback. Only when forced to correct for the error after movement completion did participants loose knowledge about the remaining error, indicating that random errors can only be accessed offline. When correcting, participants applied the optimal correction gain, a weighting factor between perceptual and motor noise, minimizing end-point variance. Together these results show that humans optimally combine direct information about sensorimotor noise in the system (the current random error), with indirect knowledge about the variance of the perceptual and motor noise distributions. Yet, they only appear to do so offline after movement completion, not while the movement is still in progress, suggesting that during movement proprioceptive information is less precise.
Erscheinungsjahr
2013
Zeitschriftentitel
PLOS ONE
Band
8
Ausgabe
10
Art.-Nr.
e78757
ISSN
1932-6203
eISSN
1932-6203
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2624805
Zitieren
van Dam L, Ernst MO. Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance. PLOS ONE. 2013;8(10): e78757.
van Dam, L., & Ernst, M. O. (2013). Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance. PLOS ONE, 8(10), e78757. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078757
van Dam, Loes, and Ernst, Marc O. 2013. “Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance”. PLOS ONE 8 (10): e78757.
van Dam, L., and Ernst, M. O. (2013). Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance. PLOS ONE 8:e78757.
van Dam, L., & Ernst, M.O., 2013. Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance. PLOS ONE, 8(10): e78757.
L. van Dam and M.O. Ernst, “Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance”, PLOS ONE, vol. 8, 2013, : e78757.
van Dam, L., Ernst, M.O.: Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance. PLOS ONE. 8, : e78757 (2013).
van Dam, Loes, and Ernst, Marc O. “Knowing Each Random Error of Our Ways, but Hardly Correcting for It: an Instance of Optimal Performance”. PLOS ONE 8.10 (2013): e78757.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Copyright Statement:
Dieses Objekt ist durch das Urheberrecht und/oder verwandte Schutzrechte geschützt. [...]
Volltext(e)
Access Level
Open Access
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-06T09:18:18Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
8668bf6fb98419db935c5f8027471980
Link(s) zu Volltext(en)
Access Level
Closed Access
Daten bereitgestellt von European Bioinformatics Institute (EBI)
5 Zitationen in Europe PMC
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Contrasting effects of adaptation to a visuomotor rotation on explicit and implicit measures of sensory coupling.
Rand MK, Heuer H., Psychol Res 83(5), 2019
PMID: 29058087
Rand MK, Heuer H., Psychol Res 83(5), 2019
PMID: 29058087
Did I do that? Detecting a perturbation to visual feedback in a reaching task.
Gaffin-Cahn E, Hudson TE, Landy MS., J Vis 19(1), 2019
PMID: 30640373
Gaffin-Cahn E, Hudson TE, Landy MS., J Vis 19(1), 2019
PMID: 30640373
Target Uncertainty Mediates Sensorimotor Error Correction.
Acerbi L, Vijayakumar S, Wolpert DM., PLoS One 12(1), 2017
PMID: 28129323
Acerbi L, Vijayakumar S, Wolpert DM., PLoS One 12(1), 2017
PMID: 28129323
Haptic adaptation to slant: No transfer between exploration modes.
van Dam LC, Plaisier MA, Glowania C, Ernst MO., Sci Rep 6(), 2016
PMID: 27698392
van Dam LC, Plaisier MA, Glowania C, Ernst MO., Sci Rep 6(), 2016
PMID: 27698392
Continuous psychophysics: Target-tracking to measure visual sensitivity.
Bonnen K, Burge J, Yates J, Pillow J, Cormack LK., J Vis 15(3), 2015
PMID: 25795437
Bonnen K, Burge J, Yates J, Pillow J, Cormack LK., J Vis 15(3), 2015
PMID: 25795437
28 References
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Noise in the nervous system.
Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9(4), 2008
PMID: 18319728
Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9(4), 2008
PMID: 18319728
Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion.
Ernst MO, Banks MS., Nature 415(6870), 2002
PMID: 11807554
Ernst MO, Banks MS., Nature 415(6870), 2002
PMID: 11807554
Shape from texture: ideal observers and human psychophysics.
Blake A, Bulthoff HH, Sheinberg D., Vision Res. 33(12), 1993
PMID: 8236859
Blake A, Bulthoff HH, Sheinberg D., Vision Res. 33(12), 1993
PMID: 8236859
Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion.
Landy MS, Maloney LT, Johnston EB, Young M., Vision Res. 35(3), 1995
PMID: 7892735
Landy MS, Maloney LT, Johnston EB, Young M., Vision Res. 35(3), 1995
PMID: 7892735
Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model.
van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, Gon JJ., J. Neurophysiol. 81(3), 1999
PMID: 10085361
van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, Gon JJ., J. Neurophysiol. 81(3), 1999
PMID: 10085361
System identification applied to a visuomotor task: near-optimal human performance in a noisy changing task.
Baddeley RJ, Ingram HA, Miall RC., J. Neurosci. 23(7), 2003
PMID: 12684493
Baddeley RJ, Ingram HA, Miall RC., J. Neurosci. 23(7), 2003
PMID: 12684493
Eye movements and the perception of a clear and stable visual world.
Martinez-Conde S, Krauzlis R, Miller J, Morrone C, Williams D, Kowler E., J Vis 8(14), 2008
PMID: 19350710
Martinez-Conde S, Krauzlis R, Miller J, Morrone C, Williams D, Kowler E., J Vis 8(14), 2008
PMID: 19350710
Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning.
Kording KP, Wolpert DM., Nature 427(6971), 2004
PMID: 14724638
Kording KP, Wolpert DM., Nature 427(6971), 2004
PMID: 14724638
A Bayesian view of motor adaptation
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2002
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2002
Is the cerebellum a smith predictor?
Miall RC, Weir DJ, Wolpert DM, Stein JF., J Mot Behav 25(3), 1993
PMID: 12581990
Miall RC, Weir DJ, Wolpert DM, Stein JF., J Mot Behav 25(3), 1993
PMID: 12581990
An internal model for sensorimotor integration.
Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI., Science 269(5232), 1995
PMID: 7569931
Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI., Science 269(5232), 1995
PMID: 7569931
Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning.
Harris CM, Wolpert DM., Nature 394(6695), 1998
PMID: 9723616
Harris CM, Wolpert DM., Nature 394(6695), 1998
PMID: 9723616
Controlling the statistics of action: obstacle avoidance.
Hamilton AF, Wolpert DM., J. Neurophysiol. 87(5), 2002
PMID: 11976380
Hamilton AF, Wolpert DM., J. Neurophysiol. 87(5), 2002
PMID: 11976380
Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination.
Todorov E, Jordan MI., Nat. Neurosci. 5(11), 2002
PMID: 12404008
Todorov E, Jordan MI., Nat. Neurosci. 5(11), 2002
PMID: 12404008
Motor learning is optimally tuned to the properties of motor noise.
van Beers RJ., Neuron 63(3), 2009
PMID: 19679079
van Beers RJ., Neuron 63(3), 2009
PMID: 19679079
Obstacle avoidance and a perturbation sensitivity model for motor planning.
Sabes PN, Jordan MI., J. Neurosci. 17(18), 1997
PMID: 9278546
Sabes PN, Jordan MI., J. Neurosci. 17(18), 1997
PMID: 9278546
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Where was my arm again? Memory-based matching of proprioceptive targets is enhanced by increased target presentation time.
Goble DJ, Noble BC, Brown SH., Neurosci. Lett. 481(1), 2010
PMID: 20600603
Goble DJ, Noble BC, Brown SH., Neurosci. Lett. 481(1), 2010
PMID: 20600603
Sensory and motor interfering influences on somatosensory evoked potentials.
Cheron G, Dan B, Borenstein S., J Clin Neurophysiol 17(3), 2000
PMID: 10928640
Cheron G, Dan B, Borenstein S., J Clin Neurophysiol 17(3), 2000
PMID: 10928640
Sensory perception during movement in man.
Chapman CE, Bushnell MC, Miron D, Duncan GH, Lund JP., Exp Brain Res 68(3), 1987
PMID: 3691723
Chapman CE, Bushnell MC, Miron D, Duncan GH, Lund JP., Exp Brain Res 68(3), 1987
PMID: 3691723
Perception of vibrotactile stimuli during motor activity in human subjects.
Post LJ, Zompa IC, Chapman CE., Exp Brain Res 100(1), 1994
PMID: 7813639
Post LJ, Zompa IC, Chapman CE., Exp Brain Res 100(1), 1994
PMID: 7813639
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee.
Collins DF, Refshauge KM, Todd G, Gandevia SC., J. Neurophysiol. 94(3), 2005
PMID: 15917323
Collins DF, Refshauge KM, Todd G, Gandevia SC., J. Neurophysiol. 94(3), 2005
PMID: 15917323
Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the human central nervous system.
Edin BB, Johansson N., J. Physiol. (Lond.) 487(1), 1995
PMID: 7473253
Edin BB, Johansson N., J. Physiol. (Lond.) 487(1), 1995
PMID: 7473253
Cutaneous afferents provide information about knee joint movements in humans.
Edin B., J. Physiol. (Lond.) 531(Pt 1), 2001
PMID: 11179411
Edin B., J. Physiol. (Lond.) 531(Pt 1), 2001
PMID: 11179411
Optimal compensation for changes in task-relevant movement variability
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2005
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2005
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 0
The psychometric function: I. fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2001
AUTHOR UNKNOWN, 2001
Export
Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen
Web of Science
Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®Quellen
PMID: 24205308
PubMed | Europe PMC
Suchen in