Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions
Herwig A, Waszak F (2012)
Frontiers in Psychology 3: 444.
Zeitschriftenaufsatz
| Veröffentlicht | Englisch
Download
Autor*in
Herwig, ArvidUniBi ;
Waszak, Florian
Einrichtung
Abstract / Bemerkung
According to ideomotor theory, action-effect associations are crucial for voluntary action control. Recently, a number of studies started to investigate the conditions that mediate the acquisition and application of action-effect associations by comparing actions carried out in response to exogenous stimuli (stimulus-based) with actions selected endogenously (intention-based). There is evidence that the acquisition and/or application of action-effect associations is boosted when acting in an intention-based action mode. For instance, bidirectional action-effect associations were diagnosed in a forced choice test phase if participants previously experienced action-effect couplings in an intention-based but not in a stimulus-based action mode. The present study aims at investigating effects of the action mode on action-effect associations in more detail. In a series of experiments, we compared the strength and durability of short-term action-effect associations (binding) immediately following intention- as well as stimulus-based actions. Moreover, long-term action-effect associations (learning) were assessed in a subsequent test phase. Our results show short-term action-effect associations of equal strength and durability for both action modes. However, replicating previous results, long-term associations were observed only following intention-based actions. These findings indicate that the effect of the action mode on long-term associations cannot merely be a result of accumulated short-term action-effect bindings. Instead, only those episodic bindings are selectively perpetuated or retrieved that integrate action-relevant aspects of the processing event, i.e., in case of intention-based actions, the link between action and ensuing effect.
Erscheinungsjahr
2012
Zeitschriftentitel
Frontiers in Psychology
Band
3
Art.-Nr.
444
ISSN
1664-1078
eISSN
1664-1078
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2531235
Zitieren
Herwig A, Waszak F. Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3: 444.
Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2012). Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 444. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00444
Herwig, Arvid, and Waszak, Florian. 2012. “Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions”. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 444.
Herwig, A., and Waszak, F. (2012). Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology 3:444.
Herwig, A., & Waszak, F., 2012. Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3: 444.
A. Herwig and F. Waszak, “Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions”, Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 3, 2012, : 444.
Herwig, A., Waszak, F.: Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology. 3, : 444 (2012).
Herwig, Arvid, and Waszak, Florian. “Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions”. Frontiers in Psychology 3 (2012): 444.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Copyright Statement:
Dieses Objekt ist durch das Urheberrecht und/oder verwandte Schutzrechte geschützt. [...]
Volltext(e)
Access Level
Open Access
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-06T09:18:07Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
2b17cd55b4d491149b47021f453ab7a1
Daten bereitgestellt von European Bioinformatics Institute (EBI)
27 Zitationen in Europe PMC
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Distract yourself: prediction of salient distractors by own actions and external cues.
Havlíček O, Müller HJ, Wykowska A., Psychol Res 83(1), 2019
PMID: 30588545
Havlíček O, Müller HJ, Wykowska A., Psychol Res 83(1), 2019
PMID: 30588545
Lost time: Bindings do not represent temporal order information.
Moeller B, Frings C., Psychon Bull Rev 26(1), 2019
PMID: 29869024
Moeller B, Frings C., Psychon Bull Rev 26(1), 2019
PMID: 29869024
Action-effects enhance explicit sequential learning.
Esser S, Haider H., Psychol Res 82(6), 2018
PMID: 28623404
Esser S, Haider H., Psychol Res 82(6), 2018
PMID: 28623404
Why free choices take longer than forced choices: evidence from response threshold manipulations.
Naefgen C, Dambacher M, Janczyk M., Psychol Res 82(6), 2018
PMID: 28776264
Naefgen C, Dambacher M, Janczyk M., Psychol Res 82(6), 2018
PMID: 28776264
Free choice tasks as random generation tasks: an investigation through working memory manipulations.
Naefgen C, Janczyk M., Exp Brain Res 236(8), 2018
PMID: 29850924
Naefgen C, Janczyk M., Exp Brain Res 236(8), 2018
PMID: 29850924
Dissecting stimulus-response binding effects: Grouping by color separately impacts integration and retrieval processes.
Laub R, Frings C, Moeller B., Atten Percept Psychophys 80(6), 2018
PMID: 29687358
Laub R, Frings C, Moeller B., Atten Percept Psychophys 80(6), 2018
PMID: 29687358
Influence of verbal instructions on effect-based action control.
Eder AB, Dignath D., Psychol Res 81(2), 2017
PMID: 26847335
Eder AB, Dignath D., Psychol Res 81(2), 2017
PMID: 26847335
Action-Effect Associations in Voluntary and Cued Task-Switching.
Sommer A, Lukas S., Front Psychol 8(), 2017
PMID: 29387027
Sommer A, Lukas S., Front Psychol 8(), 2017
PMID: 29387027
Dissociation of binding and learning processes.
Moeller B, Frings C., Atten Percept Psychophys 79(8), 2017
PMID: 28752283
Moeller B, Frings C., Atten Percept Psychophys 79(8), 2017
PMID: 28752283
The effect of SNARC compatibility on perceptual accuracy: evidence from object substitution masking.
Huffman G, Pratt J., Psychol Res 80(4), 2016
PMID: 26067890
Huffman G, Pratt J., Psychol Res 80(4), 2016
PMID: 26067890
The benefit of no choice: goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing.
Janczyk M, Dambacher M, Bieleke M, Gollwitzer PM., Psychol Res 79(2), 2015
PMID: 24619532
Janczyk M, Dambacher M, Bieleke M, Gollwitzer PM., Psychol Res 79(2), 2015
PMID: 24619532
No differences in dual-task costs between forced- and free-choice tasks.
Janczyk M, Nolden S, Jolicoeur P., Psychol Res 79(3), 2015
PMID: 24947757
Janczyk M, Nolden S, Jolicoeur P., Psychol Res 79(3), 2015
PMID: 24947757
Linking perception and action by structure or process? Toward an integrative perspective.
Herwig A., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 52(), 2015
PMID: 25732773
Herwig A., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 52(), 2015
PMID: 25732773
Influence of action-effect associations acquired by ideomotor learning on imitation.
Bunlon F, Marshall PJ, Quandt LC, Bouquet CA., PLoS One 10(3), 2015
PMID: 25793755
Bunlon F, Marshall PJ, Quandt LC, Bouquet CA., PLoS One 10(3), 2015
PMID: 25793755
Goal-Directed Resilience in Training (GRIT): A Biopsychosocial Model of Self-Regulation, Executive Functions, and Personal Growth (Eudaimonia) in Evocative Contexts of PTSD, Obesity, and Chronic Pain.
Kent M, Rivers CT, Wrenn G., Behav Sci (Basel) 5(2), 2015
PMID: 26039013
Kent M, Rivers CT, Wrenn G., Behav Sci (Basel) 5(2), 2015
PMID: 26039013
On the durability of bindings between responses and response-irrelevant stimuli.
Frings C, Moeller B, Horner AJ., Acta Psychol (Amst) 161(), 2015
PMID: 26340042
Frings C, Moeller B, Horner AJ., Acta Psychol (Amst) 161(), 2015
PMID: 26340042
Neural correlates of ideomotor effect anticipations.
Pfister R, Melcher T, Kiesel A, Dechent P, Gruber O., Neuroscience 259(), 2014
PMID: 24333210
Pfister R, Melcher T, Kiesel A, Dechent P, Gruber O., Neuroscience 259(), 2014
PMID: 24333210
Good vibrations? Vibrotactile self-stimulation reveals anticipation of body-related action effects in motor control.
Pfister R, Janczyk M, Gressmann M, Fournier LR, Kunde W., Exp Brain Res 232(3), 2014
PMID: 24337232
Pfister R, Janczyk M, Gressmann M, Fournier LR, Kunde W., Exp Brain Res 232(3), 2014
PMID: 24337232
Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance.
Janczyk M, Pfister R, Hommel B, Kunde W., Cognition 132(1), 2014
PMID: 24747873
Janczyk M, Pfister R, Hommel B, Kunde W., Cognition 132(1), 2014
PMID: 24747873
Expectation mismatch: differences between self-generated and cue-induced expectations.
Gaschler R, Schwager S, Umbach VJ, Frensch PA, Schubert T., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 46 Pt 1(), 2014
PMID: 24971824
Gaschler R, Schwager S, Umbach VJ, Frensch PA, Schubert T., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 46 Pt 1(), 2014
PMID: 24971824
Response selection difficulty modulates the behavioral impact of rapidly learnt action effects.
Wolfensteller U, Ruge H., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 25566108
Wolfensteller U, Ruge H., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 25566108
Working memory and its relation to deterministic sequence learning.
Martini M, Furtner MR, Sachse P., PLoS One 8(2), 2013
PMID: 23409148
Martini M, Furtner MR, Sachse P., PLoS One 8(2), 2013
PMID: 23409148
Editorial: Action effects in perception and action.
Pfister R, Janczyk M, Kunde W., Front Psychol 4(), 2013
PMID: 23653609
Pfister R, Janczyk M, Kunde W., Front Psychol 4(), 2013
PMID: 23653609
Side effect of acting on the world: acquisition of action-outcome statistic relation alters visual interpretation of action outcome.
Kawabe T., Front Hum Neurosci 7(), 2013
PMID: 24093017
Kawabe T., Front Hum Neurosci 7(), 2013
PMID: 24093017
Instant attraction: immediate action-effect bindings occur for both, stimulus- and goal-driven actions.
Janczyk M, Heinemann A, Pfister R., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23112787
Janczyk M, Heinemann A, Pfister R., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23112787
What I Say is What I Get: Stronger Effects of Self-Generated vs. Cue-Induced Expectations in Event-Related Potentials.
Kemper M, Umbach VJ, Schwager S, Gaschler R, Frensch PA, Stürmer B., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23403896
Kemper M, Umbach VJ, Schwager S, Gaschler R, Frensch PA, Stürmer B., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23403896
Instructed task demands and utilization of action effect anticipation.
Gaschler R, Nattkemper D., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23431066
Gaschler R, Nattkemper D., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23431066
39 References
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Learning at any rate: action-effect learning for stimulus-based actions.
Pfister R, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J., Psychol Res 75(1), 2010
PMID: 20490862
Pfister R, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J., Psychol Res 75(1), 2010
PMID: 20490862
Binding in voluntary action control.
Nattkemper D, Ziessler M, Frensch PA., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34(7), 2009
PMID: 20036685
Nattkemper D, Ziessler M, Frensch PA., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34(7), 2009
PMID: 20036685
On the timescale of stimulus-based action-effect learning.
Wolfensteller U, Ruge H., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 64(7), 2011
PMID: 21416458
Wolfensteller U, Ruge H., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 64(7), 2011
PMID: 21416458
A review of contemporary ideomotor theory.
Shin YK, Proctor RW, Capaldi EJ., Psychol Bull 136(6), 2010
PMID: 20822210
Shin YK, Proctor RW, Capaldi EJ., Psychol Bull 136(6), 2010
PMID: 20822210
Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations.
Pfister R, Kiesel A, Melcher T., Acta Psychol (Amst) 135(3), 2010
PMID: 20875631
Pfister R, Kiesel A, Melcher T., Acta Psychol (Amst) 135(3), 2010
PMID: 20875631
Action effect anticipation: neurophysiological basis and functional consequences.
Waszak F, Cardoso-Leite P, Hughes G., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36(2), 2011
PMID: 22108008
Waszak F, Cardoso-Leite P, Hughes G., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36(2), 2011
PMID: 22108008
Instant attraction: immediate action-effect bindings occur for both, stimulus- and goal-driven actions.
Janczyk M, Heinemann A, Pfister R., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23112787
Janczyk M, Heinemann A, Pfister R., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23112787
Harleß' Apparatus of Will: 150 years later.
Pfister R, Janczyk M., Psychol Res 76(5), 2011
PMID: 21748464
Pfister R, Janczyk M., Psychol Res 76(5), 2011
PMID: 21748464
Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology?
Barlow HB., Perception 1(4), 1972
PMID: 4377168
Barlow HB., Perception 1(4), 1972
PMID: 4377168
Hippocampal mediation of stimulus representation: a computational theory.
Gluck MA, Myers CE., Hippocampus 3(4), 1993
PMID: 8269040
Gluck MA, Myers CE., Hippocampus 3(4), 1993
PMID: 8269040
Cerebral structures participating in motor preparation in humans: a positron emission tomography study.
Deiber MP, Ibanez V, Sadato N, Hallett M., J. Neurophysiol. 75(1), 1996
PMID: 8822554
Deiber MP, Ibanez V, Sadato N, Hallett M., J. Neurophysiol. 75(1), 1996
PMID: 8822554
Blocking and backward blocking involve learned inattention.
Kruschke JK, Blair NJ., Psychon Bull Rev 7(4), 2000
PMID: 11206204
Kruschke JK, Blair NJ., Psychon Bull Rev 7(4), 2000
PMID: 11206204
Is synchronized neuronal gamma activity relevant for selective attention?
Fell J, Fernandez G, Klaver P, Elger CE, Fries P., Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 42(3), 2003
PMID: 12791444
Fell J, Fernandez G, Klaver P, Elger CE, Fries P., Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 42(3), 2003
PMID: 12791444
Contiguity and contingency in action-effect learning.
Elsner B, Hommel B., Psychol Res 68(2-3), 2003
PMID: 14685854
Elsner B, Hommel B., Psychol Res 68(2-3), 2003
PMID: 14685854
Intention-based and stimulus-based mechanisms in action selection.
Waszak F, Wascher E, Keller P, Koch I, Aschersleben G, Rosenbaum DA, Prinz W., Exp Brain Res 162(3), 2004
PMID: 15599722
Waszak F, Wascher E, Keller P, Koch I, Aschersleben G, Rosenbaum DA, Prinz W., Exp Brain Res 162(3), 2004
PMID: 15599722
Cortical cell assemblies: a possible mechanism for motor programs.
Wickens J, Hyland B, Anson G., J Mot Behav 26(2), 1994
PMID: 15753061
Wickens J, Hyland B, Anson G., J Mot Behav 26(2), 1994
PMID: 15753061
Early boost and slow consolidation in motor skill learning.
Hotermans C, Peigneux P, Maertens de Noordhout A, Moonen G, Maquet P., Learn. Mem. 13(5), 2006
PMID: 16980543
Hotermans C, Peigneux P, Maertens de Noordhout A, Moonen G, Maquet P., Learn. Mem. 13(5), 2006
PMID: 16980543
Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions.
Herwig A, Prinz W, Waszak F., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 60(11), 2007
PMID: 17853217
Herwig A, Prinz W, Waszak F., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 60(11), 2007
PMID: 17853217
Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex.
Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC., Cereb. Cortex 1(1), 1991
PMID: 1822724
Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC., Cereb. Cortex 1(1), 1991
PMID: 1822724
Comparison of two forms of long-term potentiation in single hippocampal neurons.
Zalutsky RA, Nicoll RA., Science 248(4963), 1990
PMID: 2114039
Zalutsky RA, Nicoll RA., Science 248(4963), 1990
PMID: 2114039
The origins of utilization behaviour.
Shallice T, Burgess PW, Schon F, Baxter DM., Brain 112 ( Pt 6)(), 1989
PMID: 2597999
Shallice T, Burgess PW, Schon F, Baxter DM., Brain 112 ( Pt 6)(), 1989
PMID: 2597999
'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes.
Lhermitte F., Brain 106 (Pt 2)(), 1983
PMID: 6850269
Lhermitte F., Brain 106 (Pt 2)(), 1983
PMID: 6850269
Movement-related potentials preceding voluntary movement are modulated by the mode of movement selection.
Praamstra P, Stegeman DF, Horstink MW, Brunia CH, Cools AR., Exp Brain Res 103(3), 1995
PMID: 7789449
Praamstra P, Stegeman DF, Horstink MW, Brunia CH, Cools AR., Exp Brain Res 103(3), 1995
PMID: 7789449
A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus.
Bliss TV, Collingridge GL., Nature 361(6407), 1993
PMID: 8421494
Bliss TV, Collingridge GL., Nature 361(6407), 1993
PMID: 8421494
The what and why of binding: the modeler's perspective.
von der Malsburg C., Neuron 24(1), 1999
PMID: 10677030
von der Malsburg C., Neuron 24(1), 1999
PMID: 10677030
Effect anticipation and action control.
Elsner B, Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27(1), 2001
PMID: 11248937
Elsner B, Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27(1), 2001
PMID: 11248937
The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information.
Kahneman D, Treisman A, Gibbs BJ., Cogn Psychol 24(2), 1992
PMID: 1582172
Kahneman D, Treisman A, Gibbs BJ., Cogn Psychol 24(2), 1992
PMID: 1582172
Synchronicity: when you're gone I'm lost without a trace?
Wagner AD., Nat. Neurosci. 4(12), 2001
PMID: 11723457
Wagner AD., Nat. Neurosci. 4(12), 2001
PMID: 11723457
Current concepts in procedural consolidation.
Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A, Miall RC., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5(7), 2004
PMID: 15208699
Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A, Miall RC., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5(7), 2004
PMID: 15208699
Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action.
Hommel B., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 8(11), 2004
PMID: 15491903
Hommel B., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 8(11), 2004
PMID: 15491903
How much attention does an event file need?
Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31(5), 2005
PMID: 16262499
Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31(5), 2005
PMID: 16262499
What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration.
Colzato LS, Raffone A, Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(3), 2006
PMID: 16822133
Colzato LS, Raffone A, Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(3), 2006
PMID: 16822133
The role of the preSMA and the rostral cingulate zone in internally selected actions.
Mueller VA, Brass M, Waszak F, Prinz W., Neuroimage 37(4), 2007
PMID: 17681798
Mueller VA, Brass M, Waszak F, Prinz W., Neuroimage 37(4), 2007
PMID: 17681798
The microgenesis of action-effect binding.
Dutzi IB, Hommel B., Psychol Res 73(3), 2008
PMID: 18810487
Dutzi IB, Hommel B., Psychol Res 73(3), 2008
PMID: 18810487
Intention and attention in ideomotor learning.
Herwig A, Waszak F., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 62(2), 2008
PMID: 18932052
Herwig A, Waszak F., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 62(2), 2008
PMID: 18932052
Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will.
Haggard P., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9(12), 2008
PMID: 19020512
Haggard P., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9(12), 2008
PMID: 19020512
Action-effect associations revealed by eye movements.
Herwig A, Horstmann G., Psychon Bull Rev 18(3), 2011
PMID: 21327975
Herwig A, Horstmann G., Psychon Bull Rev 18(3), 2011
PMID: 21327975
Export
Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen
Web of Science
Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®Quellen
PMID: 23112785
PubMed | Europe PMC
Suchen in