Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions

Herwig A, Waszak F (2012)
Frontiers in Psychology 3: 444.

Zeitschriftenaufsatz | Veröffentlicht | Englisch
 
Download
OA
Autor*in
Herwig, ArvidUniBi ; Waszak, Florian
Abstract / Bemerkung
According to ideomotor theory, action-effect associations are crucial for voluntary action control. Recently, a number of studies started to investigate the conditions that mediate the acquisition and application of action-effect associations by comparing actions carried out in response to exogenous stimuli (stimulus-based) with actions selected endogenously (intention-based). There is evidence that the acquisition and/or application of action-effect associations is boosted when acting in an intention-based action mode. For instance, bidirectional action-effect associations were diagnosed in a forced choice test phase if participants previously experienced action-effect couplings in an intention-based but not in a stimulus-based action mode. The present study aims at investigating effects of the action mode on action-effect associations in more detail. In a series of experiments, we compared the strength and durability of short-term action-effect associations (binding) immediately following intention- as well as stimulus-based actions. Moreover, long-term action-effect associations (learning) were assessed in a subsequent test phase. Our results show short-term action-effect associations of equal strength and durability for both action modes. However, replicating previous results, long-term associations were observed only following intention-based actions. These findings indicate that the effect of the action mode on long-term associations cannot merely be a result of accumulated short-term action-effect bindings. Instead, only those episodic bindings are selectively perpetuated or retrieved that integrate action-relevant aspects of the processing event, i.e., in case of intention-based actions, the link between action and ensuing effect.
Erscheinungsjahr
2012
Zeitschriftentitel
Frontiers in Psychology
Band
3
Art.-Nr.
444
ISSN
1664-1078
eISSN
1664-1078
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2531235

Zitieren

Herwig A, Waszak F. Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3: 444.
Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2012). Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 444. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00444
Herwig, Arvid, and Waszak, Florian. 2012. “Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions”. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 444.
Herwig, A., and Waszak, F. (2012). Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology 3:444.
Herwig, A., & Waszak, F., 2012. Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3: 444.
A. Herwig and F. Waszak, “Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions”, Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 3, 2012, : 444.
Herwig, A., Waszak, F.: Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions. Frontiers in Psychology. 3, : 444 (2012).
Herwig, Arvid, and Waszak, Florian. “Action-effect bindings and ideomotor learning in intention- and stimulus-based actions”. Frontiers in Psychology 3 (2012): 444.
Alle Dateien verfügbar unter der/den folgenden Lizenz(en):
Copyright Statement:
Dieses Objekt ist durch das Urheberrecht und/oder verwandte Schutzrechte geschützt. [...]
Volltext(e)
Access Level
OA Open Access
Zuletzt Hochgeladen
2019-09-06T09:18:07Z
MD5 Prüfsumme
2b17cd55b4d491149b47021f453ab7a1


27 Zitationen in Europe PMC

Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.

Distract yourself: prediction of salient distractors by own actions and external cues.
Havlíček O, Müller HJ, Wykowska A., Psychol Res 83(1), 2019
PMID: 30588545
Lost time: Bindings do not represent temporal order information.
Moeller B, Frings C., Psychon Bull Rev 26(1), 2019
PMID: 29869024
Action-effects enhance explicit sequential learning.
Esser S, Haider H., Psychol Res 82(6), 2018
PMID: 28623404
Influence of verbal instructions on effect-based action control.
Eder AB, Dignath D., Psychol Res 81(2), 2017
PMID: 26847335
Action-Effect Associations in Voluntary and Cued Task-Switching.
Sommer A, Lukas S., Front Psychol 8(), 2017
PMID: 29387027
Dissociation of binding and learning processes.
Moeller B, Frings C., Atten Percept Psychophys 79(8), 2017
PMID: 28752283
The benefit of no choice: goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing.
Janczyk M, Dambacher M, Bieleke M, Gollwitzer PM., Psychol Res 79(2), 2015
PMID: 24619532
No differences in dual-task costs between forced- and free-choice tasks.
Janczyk M, Nolden S, Jolicoeur P., Psychol Res 79(3), 2015
PMID: 24947757
Influence of action-effect associations acquired by ideomotor learning on imitation.
Bunlon F, Marshall PJ, Quandt LC, Bouquet CA., PLoS One 10(3), 2015
PMID: 25793755
On the durability of bindings between responses and response-irrelevant stimuli.
Frings C, Moeller B, Horner AJ., Acta Psychol (Amst) 161(), 2015
PMID: 26340042
Neural correlates of ideomotor effect anticipations.
Pfister R, Melcher T, Kiesel A, Dechent P, Gruber O., Neuroscience 259(), 2014
PMID: 24333210
Good vibrations? Vibrotactile self-stimulation reveals anticipation of body-related action effects in motor control.
Pfister R, Janczyk M, Gressmann M, Fournier LR, Kunde W., Exp Brain Res 232(3), 2014
PMID: 24337232
Expectation mismatch: differences between self-generated and cue-induced expectations.
Gaschler R, Schwager S, Umbach VJ, Frensch PA, Schubert T., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 46 Pt 1(), 2014
PMID: 24971824
Working memory and its relation to deterministic sequence learning.
Martini M, Furtner MR, Sachse P., PLoS One 8(2), 2013
PMID: 23409148
Editorial: Action effects in perception and action.
Pfister R, Janczyk M, Kunde W., Front Psychol 4(), 2013
PMID: 23653609
What I Say is What I Get: Stronger Effects of Self-Generated vs. Cue-Induced Expectations in Event-Related Potentials.
Kemper M, Umbach VJ, Schwager S, Gaschler R, Frensch PA, Stürmer B., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23403896
Instructed task demands and utilization of action effect anticipation.
Gaschler R, Nattkemper D., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23431066

39 References

Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.

Learning at any rate: action-effect learning for stimulus-based actions.
Pfister R, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J., Psychol Res 75(1), 2010
PMID: 20490862
Binding in voluntary action control.
Nattkemper D, Ziessler M, Frensch PA., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34(7), 2009
PMID: 20036685
On the timescale of stimulus-based action-effect learning.
Wolfensteller U, Ruge H., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 64(7), 2011
PMID: 21416458
A review of contemporary ideomotor theory.
Shin YK, Proctor RW, Capaldi EJ., Psychol Bull 136(6), 2010
PMID: 20822210
Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations.
Pfister R, Kiesel A, Melcher T., Acta Psychol (Amst) 135(3), 2010
PMID: 20875631
Action effect anticipation: neurophysiological basis and functional consequences.
Waszak F, Cardoso-Leite P, Hughes G., Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36(2), 2011
PMID: 22108008
Harleß' Apparatus of Will: 150 years later.
Pfister R, Janczyk M., Psychol Res 76(5), 2011
PMID: 21748464
Cerebral structures participating in motor preparation in humans: a positron emission tomography study.
Deiber MP, Ibanez V, Sadato N, Hallett M., J. Neurophysiol. 75(1), 1996
PMID: 8822554
Blocking and backward blocking involve learned inattention.
Kruschke JK, Blair NJ., Psychon Bull Rev 7(4), 2000
PMID: 11206204
Is synchronized neuronal gamma activity relevant for selective attention?
Fell J, Fernandez G, Klaver P, Elger CE, Fries P., Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 42(3), 2003
PMID: 12791444
Contiguity and contingency in action-effect learning.
Elsner B, Hommel B., Psychol Res 68(2-3), 2003
PMID: 14685854
Intention-based and stimulus-based mechanisms in action selection.
Waszak F, Wascher E, Keller P, Koch I, Aschersleben G, Rosenbaum DA, Prinz W., Exp Brain Res 162(3), 2004
PMID: 15599722
Cortical cell assemblies: a possible mechanism for motor programs.
Wickens J, Hyland B, Anson G., J Mot Behav 26(2), 1994
PMID: 15753061
Early boost and slow consolidation in motor skill learning.
Hotermans C, Peigneux P, Maertens de Noordhout A, Moonen G, Maquet P., Learn. Mem. 13(5), 2006
PMID: 16980543
Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions.
Herwig A, Prinz W, Waszak F., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 60(11), 2007
PMID: 17853217
Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex.
Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC., Cereb. Cortex 1(1), 1991
PMID: 1822724
Comparison of two forms of long-term potentiation in single hippocampal neurons.
Zalutsky RA, Nicoll RA., Science 248(4963), 1990
PMID: 2114039
The origins of utilization behaviour.
Shallice T, Burgess PW, Schon F, Baxter DM., Brain 112 ( Pt 6)(), 1989
PMID: 2597999
Movement-related potentials preceding voluntary movement are modulated by the mode of movement selection.
Praamstra P, Stegeman DF, Horstink MW, Brunia CH, Cools AR., Exp Brain Res 103(3), 1995
PMID: 7789449
A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus.
Bliss TV, Collingridge GL., Nature 361(6407), 1993
PMID: 8421494
The binding problem.
Treisman A., Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6(2), 1996
PMID: 8725958
Memory--a century of consolidation.
McGaugh JL., Science 287(5451), 2000
PMID: 10634773
The what and why of binding: the modeler's perspective.
von der Malsburg C., Neuron 24(1), 1999
PMID: 10677030
Effect anticipation and action control.
Elsner B, Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27(1), 2001
PMID: 11248937
The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information.
Kahneman D, Treisman A, Gibbs BJ., Cogn Psychol 24(2), 1992
PMID: 1582172
Synchronicity: when you're gone I'm lost without a trace?
Wagner AD., Nat. Neurosci. 4(12), 2001
PMID: 11723457
Current concepts in procedural consolidation.
Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A, Miall RC., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5(7), 2004
PMID: 15208699
Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action.
Hommel B., Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 8(11), 2004
PMID: 15491903
How much attention does an event file need?
Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31(5), 2005
PMID: 16262499
What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration.
Colzato LS, Raffone A, Hommel B., J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(3), 2006
PMID: 16822133
The role of the preSMA and the rostral cingulate zone in internally selected actions.
Mueller VA, Brass M, Waszak F, Prinz W., Neuroimage 37(4), 2007
PMID: 17681798
The microgenesis of action-effect binding.
Dutzi IB, Hommel B., Psychol Res 73(3), 2008
PMID: 18810487
Intention and attention in ideomotor learning.
Herwig A, Waszak F., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 62(2), 2008
PMID: 18932052
Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will.
Haggard P., Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9(12), 2008
PMID: 19020512
Action-effect associations revealed by eye movements.
Herwig A, Horstmann G., Psychon Bull Rev 18(3), 2011
PMID: 21327975
Export

Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen

Open Data PUB

Web of Science

Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®
Quellen

PMID: 23112785
PubMed | Europe PMC

Suchen in

Google Scholar