The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory
Spiegel MA, Koester D, Weigelt M, Schack T (2012)
Neuroscience Letters 509(2): 82-86.
Zeitschriftenaufsatz
| Veröffentlicht | Englisch
Download
Es wurden keine Dateien hochgeladen. Nur Publikationsnachweis!
Einrichtung
Abstract / Bemerkung
How much cognitive effort does it take to change a movement plan? In previous studies, it has been shown that humans plan and represent actions in advance, but it remains unclear whether or not action planning and verbal working memory share cognitive resources. Using a novel experimental paradigm, we combined in two experiments a grasp-to-place task with a verbal working memory task. Participants planned a placing movement toward one of two target positions and subsequently encoded and maintained visually presented letters. Both experiments revealed that re-planning the intended action reduced letter recall performance; execution time, however, was not influenced by action modifications. The results of Experiment 2 suggest that the action's interference with verbal working memory arose during the planning rather than the execution phase of the movement. Together, our results strongly suggest that movement planning and verbal working memory share common cognitive resources.
Stichworte
Action modification;
Action planning;
Grasping;
Verbal working memory;
Dual-task
Erscheinungsjahr
2012
Zeitschriftentitel
Neuroscience Letters
Band
509
Ausgabe
2
Seite(n)
82-86
ISSN
0304-3940
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2459985
Zitieren
Spiegel MA, Koester D, Weigelt M, Schack T. The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory. Neuroscience Letters. 2012;509(2):82-86.
Spiegel, M. A., Koester, D., Weigelt, M., & Schack, T. (2012). The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory. Neuroscience Letters, 509(2), 82-86. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2011.12.033
Spiegel, Marnie Ann, Koester, Dirk, Weigelt, Matthias, and Schack, Thomas. 2012. “The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory”. Neuroscience Letters 509 (2): 82-86.
Spiegel, M. A., Koester, D., Weigelt, M., and Schack, T. (2012). The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory. Neuroscience Letters 509, 82-86.
Spiegel, M.A., et al., 2012. The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory. Neuroscience Letters, 509(2), p 82-86.
M.A. Spiegel, et al., “The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory”, Neuroscience Letters, vol. 509, 2012, pp. 82-86.
Spiegel, M.A., Koester, D., Weigelt, M., Schack, T.: The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory. Neuroscience Letters. 509, 82-86 (2012).
Spiegel, Marnie Ann, Koester, Dirk, Weigelt, Matthias, and Schack, Thomas. “The costs of changing an intended action: Movement planning, but not execution, interferes with verbal working memory”. Neuroscience Letters 509.2 (2012): 82-86.
Daten bereitgestellt von European Bioinformatics Institute (EBI)
10 Zitationen in Europe PMC
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Movement Interferes with Visuospatial Working Memory during the Encoding: An ERP Study.
Gunduz Can R, Schack T, Koester D., Front Psychol 8(), 2017
PMID: 28611714
Gunduz Can R, Schack T, Koester D., Front Psychol 8(), 2017
PMID: 28611714
No Interrelation of Motor Planning and Executive Functions across Young Ages.
Wunsch K, Pfister R, Henning A, Aschersleben G, Weigelt M., Front Psychol 7(), 2016
PMID: 27462285
Wunsch K, Pfister R, Henning A, Aschersleben G, Weigelt M., Front Psychol 7(), 2016
PMID: 27462285
Action Priority: Early Neurophysiological Interaction of Conceptual and Motor Representations.
Koester D, Schack T., PLoS One 11(12), 2016
PMID: 27973539
Koester D, Schack T., PLoS One 11(12), 2016
PMID: 27973539
Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands.
Schütz C, Schack T., Front Psychol 6(), 2015
PMID: 26441734
Schütz C, Schack T., Front Psychol 6(), 2015
PMID: 26441734
Movement planning and attentional control of visuospatial working memory: evidence from a grasp-to-place task.
Spiegel MA, Koester D, Schack T., Psychol Res 78(4), 2014
PMID: 23832553
Spiegel MA, Koester D, Schack T., Psychol Res 78(4), 2014
PMID: 23832553
Are tool properties always processed automatically? The role of tool use context and task complexity.
Randerath J, Martin KR, Frey SH., Cortex 49(6), 2013
PMID: 23026760
Randerath J, Martin KR, Frey SH., Cortex 49(6), 2013
PMID: 23026760
Motor expertise modulates movement processing in working memory.
Moreau D., Acta Psychol (Amst) 142(3), 2013
PMID: 23422289
Moreau D., Acta Psychol (Amst) 142(3), 2013
PMID: 23422289
Event-related brain potentials for goal-related power grips.
Westerholz J, Schack T, Koester D., PLoS One 8(7), 2013
PMID: 23844211
Westerholz J, Schack T, Koester D., PLoS One 8(7), 2013
PMID: 23844211
Cognitive-motor interference while grasping, lifting and holding objects.
Guillery E, Mouraux A, Thonnard JL., PLoS One 8(11), 2013
PMID: 24244626
Guillery E, Mouraux A, Thonnard JL., PLoS One 8(11), 2013
PMID: 24244626
Investigating ideomotor cognition with motorvisual priming paradigms: key findings, methodological challenges, and future directions.
Thomaschke R., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23189067
Thomaschke R., Front Psychol 3(), 2012
PMID: 23189067
References
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Export
Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen
Web of Science
Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®Quellen
PMID: 22230898
PubMed | Europe PMC
Suchen in