The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity
Gepshtein S, Burge J, Ernst MO, Banks MS (2005)
Journal of Vision 5(11): 7.
Zeitschriftenaufsatz
| Veröffentlicht | Englisch
Download
Es wurden keine Dateien hochgeladen. Nur Publikationsnachweis!
Autor*in
Gepshtein, S;
Burge, J;
Ernst, Marc O.UniBi;
Banks, MS
Abstract / Bemerkung
The nervous system often combines visual and haptic information about object properties such that the combined estimate is more precise than with vision or haptics alone. We examined how the system determines when to combine the signals. Presumably, signals should not be combined when they come from different objects. The likelihood that signals come from different objects is highly correlated with the spatial separation between the signals, so we asked how the spatial separation between visual and haptic signals affects their combination. To do this, we first created conditions for each observer in which the effect of combination-the increase in discrimination precision with two modalities relative to performance with one modality-should be maximal. Then under these conditions, we presented visual and haptic stimuli separated by different spatial distances and compared human performance with predictions of a model that combined signals optimally. We found that discrimination precision was essentially optimal when the signals came from the same location, and that discrimination precision was poorer when the signals came from different locations. Thus, the mechanism of visual-haptic combination is specialized for signals that coincide in space.
Stichworte
optimality;
objects;
vision;
inter-sensory integration;
haptics;
multidimensional classification;
proximity principle;
spatial attention
Erscheinungsjahr
2005
Zeitschriftentitel
Journal of Vision
Band
5
Ausgabe
11
Seite(n)
7
ISSN
1534-7362
Page URI
https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de/record/2288057
Zitieren
Gepshtein S, Burge J, Ernst MO, Banks MS. The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity. Journal of Vision. 2005;5(11):7.
Gepshtein, S., Burge, J., Ernst, M. O., & Banks, M. S. (2005). The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity. Journal of Vision, 5(11), 7. https://doi.org/10.1167/5.11.7
Gepshtein, S, Burge, J, Ernst, Marc O., and Banks, MS. 2005. “The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity”. Journal of Vision 5 (11): 7.
Gepshtein, S., Burge, J., Ernst, M. O., and Banks, M. S. (2005). The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity. Journal of Vision 5, 7.
Gepshtein, S., et al., 2005. The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity. Journal of Vision, 5(11), p 7.
S. Gepshtein, et al., “The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity”, Journal of Vision, vol. 5, 2005, pp. 7.
Gepshtein, S., Burge, J., Ernst, M.O., Banks, M.S.: The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity. Journal of Vision. 5, 7 (2005).
Gepshtein, S, Burge, J, Ernst, Marc O., and Banks, MS. “The combination of vision and touch depends on spatial proximity”. Journal of Vision 5.11 (2005): 7.
Daten bereitgestellt von European Bioinformatics Institute (EBI)
56 Zitationen in Europe PMC
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
Grasping movements toward seen and handheld objects.
Camponogara I, Volcic R., Sci Rep 9(1), 2019
PMID: 30842478
Camponogara I, Volcic R., Sci Rep 9(1), 2019
PMID: 30842478
Optimal integration of intraneural somatosensory feedback with visual information: a single-case study.
Risso G, Valle G, Iberite F, Strauss I, Stieglitz T, Controzzi M, Clemente F, Granata G, Rossini PM, Micera S, Baud-Bovy G., Sci Rep 9(1), 2019
PMID: 31133637
Risso G, Valle G, Iberite F, Strauss I, Stieglitz T, Controzzi M, Clemente F, Granata G, Rossini PM, Micera S, Baud-Bovy G., Sci Rep 9(1), 2019
PMID: 31133637
Reliability-Weighted Integration of Audiovisual Signals Can Be Modulated by Top-down Attention.
Rohe T, Noppeney U., eNeuro 5(1), 2018
PMID: 29527567
Rohe T, Noppeney U., eNeuro 5(1), 2018
PMID: 29527567
Object discrepancy modulates feature prediction across eye movements.
Köller CP, Poth CH, Herwig A., Psychol Res (), 2018
PMID: 29387939
Köller CP, Poth CH, Herwig A., Psychol Res (), 2018
PMID: 29387939
Suboptimality in Perceptual Decision Making.
Rahnev D, Denison RN., Behav Brain Sci (), 2018
PMID: 29485020
Rahnev D, Denison RN., Behav Brain Sci (), 2018
PMID: 29485020
Optimal integration of actions and their visual effects is based on both online and prior causality evidence.
Debats NB, Heuer H., Sci Rep 8(1), 2018
PMID: 29955156
Debats NB, Heuer H., Sci Rep 8(1), 2018
PMID: 29955156
Effects of auditory feedback on movements with two-segment sequence and eye-hand coordination.
Rand MK., Exp Brain Res 236(12), 2018
PMID: 30159590
Rand MK., Exp Brain Res 236(12), 2018
PMID: 30159590
Perceptual attraction in tool use: evidence for a reliability-based weighting mechanism.
Debats NB, Ernst MO, Heuer H., J Neurophysiol 117(4), 2017
PMID: 28100656
Debats NB, Ernst MO, Heuer H., J Neurophysiol 117(4), 2017
PMID: 28100656
Optimal visual-haptic integration with articulated tools.
Takahashi C, Watt SJ., Exp Brain Res 235(5), 2017
PMID: 28214998
Takahashi C, Watt SJ., Exp Brain Res 235(5), 2017
PMID: 28214998
Trust in haptic assistance: weighting visual and haptic cues based on error history.
Gibo TL, Mugge W, Abbink DA., Exp Brain Res 235(8), 2017
PMID: 28534068
Gibo TL, Mugge W, Abbink DA., Exp Brain Res 235(8), 2017
PMID: 28534068
Finding Home: Landmark Ambiguity in Human Navigation.
Jetzschke S, Ernst MO, Froehlich J, Boeddeker N., Front Behav Neurosci 11(), 2017
PMID: 28769773
Jetzschke S, Ernst MO, Froehlich J, Boeddeker N., Front Behav Neurosci 11(), 2017
PMID: 28769773
Kinematic cross-correlation induces sensory integration across separate objects.
Debats NB, Ernst MO, Heuer H., Eur J Neurosci 46(12), 2017
PMID: 29068094
Debats NB, Ernst MO, Heuer H., Eur J Neurosci 46(12), 2017
PMID: 29068094
Attentional Resource Allocation in Visuotactile Processing Depends on the Task, But Optimal Visuotactile Integration Does Not Depend on Attentional Resources.
Wahn B, König P., Front Integr Neurosci 10(), 2016
PMID: 27013994
Wahn B, König P., Front Integr Neurosci 10(), 2016
PMID: 27013994
The Development of Audio-Visual Integration for Temporal Judgements.
Adams WJ., PLoS Comput Biol 12(4), 2016
PMID: 27078834
Adams WJ., PLoS Comput Biol 12(4), 2016
PMID: 27078834
Optimal visual-vestibular integration under conditions of conflicting intersensory motion profiles.
Butler JS, Campos JL, Bülthoff HH., Exp Brain Res 233(2), 2015
PMID: 25361642
Butler JS, Campos JL, Bülthoff HH., Exp Brain Res 233(2), 2015
PMID: 25361642
The influence of intersensory discrepancy on visuo-haptic integration is similar in 6-year-old children and adults.
Jovanovic B, Drewing K., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24523712
Jovanovic B, Drewing K., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24523712
Again, knowledge of common source fails to promote visual-haptic integration.
Misceo GF, Jackson SV, Perdue JR., Percept Mot Skills 118(1), 2014
PMID: 24724521
Misceo GF, Jackson SV, Perdue JR., Percept Mot Skills 118(1), 2014
PMID: 24724521
Visual-haptic integration with pliers and tongs: signal "weights" take account of changes in haptic sensitivity caused by different tools.
Takahashi C, Watt SJ., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24592245
Takahashi C, Watt SJ., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24592245
Visual target distance, but not visual cursor path length produces shifts in motor behavior.
Wendker N, Sack OS, Sutter C., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24672507
Wendker N, Sack OS, Sutter C., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24672507
On the 'visual' in 'audio-visual integration': a hypothesis concerning visual pathways.
Jaekl P, Pérez-Bellido A, Soto-Faraco S., Exp Brain Res 232(6), 2014
PMID: 24699769
Jaekl P, Pérez-Bellido A, Soto-Faraco S., Exp Brain Res 232(6), 2014
PMID: 24699769
Multisensory integration in action control.
Sutter C, Drewing K, Müsseler J., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24959154
Sutter C, Drewing K, Müsseler J., Front Psychol 5(), 2014
PMID: 24959154
Contributions of visual and proprioceptive information to travelled distance estimation during changing sensory congruencies.
Campos JL, Butler JS, Bülthoff HH., Exp Brain Res 232(10), 2014
PMID: 24961739
Campos JL, Butler JS, Bülthoff HH., Exp Brain Res 232(10), 2014
PMID: 24961739
Humans use predictive kinematic models to calibrate visual cues to three-dimensional surface slant.
Scarfe P, Glennerster A., J Neurosci 34(31), 2014
PMID: 25080598
Scarfe P, Glennerster A., J Neurosci 34(31), 2014
PMID: 25080598
Effects of haptic information on the perception of dynamic 3-D movement.
Umemura H., PLoS One 9(9), 2014
PMID: 25180594
Umemura H., PLoS One 9(9), 2014
PMID: 25180594
Motor-sensory recalibration modulates perceived simultaneity of cross-modal events at different distances.
Parsons BD, Novich SD, Eagleman DM., Front Psychol 4(), 2013
PMID: 23549660
Parsons BD, Novich SD, Eagleman DM., Front Psychol 4(), 2013
PMID: 23549660
Seeing and hearing a word: combining eye and ear is more efficient than combining the parts of a word.
Dubois M, Poeppel D, Pelli DG., PLoS One 8(5), 2013
PMID: 23734220
Dubois M, Poeppel D, Pelli DG., PLoS One 8(5), 2013
PMID: 23734220
Visual and haptic integration in the estimation of softness of deformable objects.
Cellini C, Kaim L, Drewing K., Iperception 4(8), 2013
PMID: 25165510
Cellini C, Kaim L, Drewing K., Iperception 4(8), 2013
PMID: 25165510
Visuo-haptic neuronal convergence demonstrated with an inversely effective pattern of BOLD activation.
Kim S, Stevenson RA, James TW., J Cogn Neurosci 24(4), 2012
PMID: 22185495
Kim S, Stevenson RA, James TW., J Cogn Neurosci 24(4), 2012
PMID: 22185495
Time gain influences adaptive visual-motor isometric force control.
Hu X, Mazich MM, Newell KM., Exp Brain Res 218(1), 2012
PMID: 22278109
Hu X, Mazich MM, Newell KM., Exp Brain Res 218(1), 2012
PMID: 22278109
Audiovisual time perception is spatially specific.
Heron J, Roach NW, Hanson JV, McGraw PV, Whitaker D., Exp Brain Res 218(3), 2012
PMID: 22367399
Heron J, Roach NW, Hanson JV, McGraw PV, Whitaker D., Exp Brain Res 218(3), 2012
PMID: 22367399
Multistability, cross-modal binding and the additivity of conjoined grouping principles.
Kubovy M, Yu M., Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 367(1591), 2012
PMID: 22371617
Kubovy M, Yu M., Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 367(1591), 2012
PMID: 22371617
Continuous evolution of statistical estimators for optimal decision-making.
Saunders I, Vijayakumar S., PLoS One 7(6), 2012
PMID: 22761657
Saunders I, Vijayakumar S., PLoS One 7(6), 2012
PMID: 22761657
Cross-modal processing in the occipito-temporal cortex: a TMS study of the Müller-Lyer illusion.
Mancini F, Bolognini N, Bricolo E, Vallar G., J Cogn Neurosci 23(8), 2011
PMID: 20807050
Mancini F, Bolognini N, Bricolo E, Vallar G., J Cogn Neurosci 23(8), 2011
PMID: 20807050
Interaction of perceptual grouping and crossmodal temporal capture in tactile apparent-motion.
Chen L, Shi Z, Müller HJ., PLoS One 6(2), 2011
PMID: 21383834
Chen L, Shi Z, Müller HJ., PLoS One 6(2), 2011
PMID: 21383834
Testing the limits of optimal integration of visual and proprioceptive information of path trajectory.
Reuschel J, Rösler F, Henriques DY, Fiehler K., Exp Brain Res 209(4), 2011
PMID: 21347659
Reuschel J, Rösler F, Henriques DY, Fiehler K., Exp Brain Res 209(4), 2011
PMID: 21347659
Visual-haptic cue integration with spatial and temporal disparity during pointing movements.
Serwe S, Körding KP, Trommershäuser J., Exp Brain Res 210(1), 2011
PMID: 21374079
Serwe S, Körding KP, Trommershäuser J., Exp Brain Res 210(1), 2011
PMID: 21374079
Human spatial orientation in non-stationary environments: relation between self-turning perception and detection of surround motion.
Jürgens R, Becker W., Exp Brain Res 215(3-4), 2011
PMID: 22006272
Jürgens R, Becker W., Exp Brain Res 215(3-4), 2011
PMID: 22006272
Multisensory integration in the Muller-Lyer illusion: from vision to haptics.
Mancini F, Bricolo E, Vallar G., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 63(4), 2010
PMID: 19728224
Mancini F, Bricolo E, Vallar G., Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 63(4), 2010
PMID: 19728224
Optimal integration of visual and proprioceptive movement information for the perception of trajectory geometry.
Reuschel J, Drewing K, Henriques DY, Rösler F, Fiehler K., Exp Brain Res 201(4), 2010
PMID: 19953230
Reuschel J, Drewing K, Henriques DY, Rösler F, Fiehler K., Exp Brain Res 201(4), 2010
PMID: 19953230
Encoding of tangential torque in responses of tactile afferent fibres innervating the fingerpad of the monkey.
Birznieks I, Wheat HE, Redmond SJ, Salo LM, Lovell NH, Goodwin AW., J Physiol 588(pt 7), 2010
PMID: 20142274
Birznieks I, Wheat HE, Redmond SJ, Salo LM, Lovell NH, Goodwin AW., J Physiol 588(pt 7), 2010
PMID: 20142274
Visualizing 3D objects from 2D cross sectional images displayed in-situ versus ex-situ.
Wu B, Klatzky RL, Stetten G., J Exp Psychol Appl 16(1), 2010
PMID: 20350043
Wu B, Klatzky RL, Stetten G., J Exp Psychol Appl 16(1), 2010
PMID: 20350043
Object-guided spatial attention in touch: holding the same object with both hands delays attentional selection.
Gillmeister H, Adler J, Forster B., J Cogn Neurosci 22(5), 2010
PMID: 19413480
Gillmeister H, Adler J, Forster B., J Cogn Neurosci 22(5), 2010
PMID: 19413480
In praise of "false" models and rich data.
Fernandes HL, Kording KP., J Mot Behav 42(6), 2010
PMID: 21184351
Fernandes HL, Kording KP., J Mot Behav 42(6), 2010
PMID: 21184351
Sensory dominance in combinations of audio, visual and haptic stimuli.
Hecht D, Reiner M., Exp Brain Res 193(2), 2009
PMID: 18985327
Hecht D, Reiner M., Exp Brain Res 193(2), 2009
PMID: 18985327
Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation?
Wei K, Körding K., J Neurophysiol 101(2), 2009
PMID: 19019979
Wei K, Körding K., J Neurophysiol 101(2), 2009
PMID: 19019979
Testing a counter-intuitive prediction of optimal cue combination.
Muller CM, Brenner E, Smeets JB., Vision Res 49(1), 2009
PMID: 18983869
Muller CM, Brenner E, Smeets JB., Vision Res 49(1), 2009
PMID: 18983869
Multisensory oddity detection as bayesian inference.
Hospedales T, Vijayakumar S., PLoS One 4(1), 2009
PMID: 19145254
Hospedales T, Vijayakumar S., PLoS One 4(1), 2009
PMID: 19145254
The role of visuohaptic experience in visually perceived depth.
Ho YX, Serwe S, Trommershäuser J, Maloney LT, Landy MS., J Neurophysiol 101(6), 2009
PMID: 19357346
Ho YX, Serwe S, Trommershäuser J, Maloney LT, Landy MS., J Neurophysiol 101(6), 2009
PMID: 19357346
Combining eye and hand in search is suboptimal.
Liesker H, Brenner E, Smeets JB., Exp Brain Res 197(4), 2009
PMID: 19590859
Liesker H, Brenner E, Smeets JB., Exp Brain Res 197(4), 2009
PMID: 19590859
When vision 'extinguishes' touch in neurologically-normal people: extending the Colavita visual dominance effect.
Hartcher-O'Brien J, Gallace A, Krings B, Koppen C, Spence C., Exp Brain Res 186(4), 2008
PMID: 18301885
Hartcher-O'Brien J, Gallace A, Krings B, Koppen C, Spence C., Exp Brain Res 186(4), 2008
PMID: 18301885
Material properties determine how force and position signals combine in haptic shape perception.
Drewing K, Wiecki TV, Ernst MO., Acta Psychol (Amst) 128(2), 2008
PMID: 18359467
Drewing K, Wiecki TV, Ernst MO., Acta Psychol (Amst) 128(2), 2008
PMID: 18359467
Knowledge about a common source can promote visual- haptic integration.
Helbig HB, Ernst MO., Perception 36(10), 2007
PMID: 18265835
Helbig HB, Ernst MO., Perception 36(10), 2007
PMID: 18265835
Optimal integration of shape information from vision and touch.
Helbig HB, Ernst MO., Exp Brain Res 179(4), 2007
PMID: 17225091
Helbig HB, Ernst MO., Exp Brain Res 179(4), 2007
PMID: 17225091
Causal inference in multisensory perception.
Körding KP, Beierholm U, Ma WJ, Quartz S, Tenenbaum JB, Shams L., PLoS One 2(9), 2007
PMID: 17895984
Körding KP, Beierholm U, Ma WJ, Quartz S, Tenenbaum JB, Shams L., PLoS One 2(9), 2007
PMID: 17895984
Resolving multisensory conflict: a strategy for balancing the costs and benefits of audio-visual integration.
Roach NW, Heron J, McGraw PV., Proc Biol Sci 273(1598), 2006
PMID: 16901835
Roach NW, Heron J, McGraw PV., Proc Biol Sci 273(1598), 2006
PMID: 16901835
28 References
Daten bereitgestellt von Europe PubMed Central.
The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration.
Alais D, Burr D., Curr. Biol. 14(3), 2004
PMID: 14761661
Alais D, Burr D., Curr. Biol. 14(3), 2004
PMID: 14761661
Bevington P, Robinson DK., 1992
Feeling what you hear: Auditory signals can modulate tactile taps perception
Bresciani JP, Ernst MO, Drewing K, Bouyer G, Maury V, Kheddar A., 2005
Bresciani JP, Ernst MO, Drewing K, Bouyer G, Maury V, Kheddar A., 2005
Coombs CH, Dawes RM, Tversky A., 1970
Duda RO, Hart PE, Stork DG., 2001
Ecological statistics of Gestalt laws for the perceptual organization of contours.
Elder JH, Goldberg RM., J Vis 2(4), 2002
PMID: 12678582
Elder JH, Goldberg RM., J Vis 2(4), 2002
PMID: 12678582
Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion.
Ernst MO, Banks MS., Nature 415(6870), 2002
PMID: 11807554
Ernst MO, Banks MS., Nature 415(6870), 2002
PMID: 11807554
Edge co-occurrence in natural images predicts contour grouping performance.
Geisler WS, Perry JS, Super BJ, Gallogly DP., Vision Res. 41(6), 2001
PMID: 11248261
Geisler WS, Perry JS, Super BJ, Gallogly DP., Vision Res. 41(6), 2001
PMID: 11248261
Viewing geometry determines how vision and haptics combine in size perception.
Gepshtein S, Banks MS., Curr. Biol. 13(6), 2003
PMID: 12646130
Gepshtein S, Banks MS., Curr. Biol. 13(6), 2003
PMID: 12646130
Combining sensory information: mandatory fusion within, but not between, senses.
Hillis JM, Ernst MO, Banks MS, Landy MS., Science 298(5598), 2002
PMID: 12446912
Hillis JM, Ernst MO, Banks MS, Landy MS., Science 298(5598), 2002
PMID: 12446912
Kanizsa G., 1979
Krantz D, Luce R, Suppes P, Tversky A., 1971
On the lawfulness of grouping by proximity.
Kubovy M, Holcombe AO, Wagemans J., Cogn Psychol 35(1), 1998
PMID: 9520318
Kubovy M, Holcombe AO, Wagemans J., Cogn Psychol 35(1), 1998
PMID: 9520318
Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion.
Landy MS, Maloney LT, Johnston EB, Young M., Vision Res. 35(3), 1995
PMID: 7892735
Landy MS, Maloney LT, Johnston EB, Young M., Vision Res. 35(3), 1995
PMID: 7892735
A reply to McDonald, J. J., Teder-Sälejärvi, W. A., & Ward, L. M. Multisensory integration and crossmodal attention effects in the human brain
Macaluso E, Frith C, Driver J., 2001
Macaluso E, Frith C, Driver J., 2001
Martin D, Fowlkes C, Tal D, Malik J., 0
The phenomenology of attention: Part 1. Color, location, orientation, and “clarity.”
Prinzmetal W, Amiri H, Allen K, Edwards T., 1998
Prinzmetal W, Amiri H, Allen K, Edwards T., 1998
Rosenblatt F., 1961
Statistics of natural images: Scaling in the woods.
Ruderman DL, Bialek W., Phys. Rev. Lett. 73(6), 1994
PMID: 10057546
Ruderman DL, Bialek W., Phys. Rev. Lett. 73(6), 1994
PMID: 10057546
Illusions. What you see is what you hear.
Shams L, Kamitani Y, Shimojo S., Nature 408(6814), 2000
PMID: 11130706
Shams L, Kamitani Y, Shimojo S., Nature 408(6814), 2000
PMID: 11130706
Exogenous spatial-cuing studies of human crossmodal attention and multisensory integration
Spence C, McDonald J, Driver J., 2004
Spence C, McDonald J, Driver J., 2004
Surface tilt (the direction of slant): a neglected psychophysical variable.
Stevens KA., Percept Psychophys 33(3), 1983
PMID: 6866683
Stevens KA., Percept Psychophys 33(3), 1983
PMID: 6866683
Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects.
Treisman A, Schmidt H., Cogn Psychol 14(1), 1982
PMID: 7053925
Treisman A, Schmidt H., Cogn Psychol 14(1), 1982
PMID: 7053925
When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation.
van Beers RJ, Wolpert DM, Haggard P., Curr. Biol. 12(10), 2002
PMID: 12015120
van Beers RJ, Wolpert DM, Haggard P., Curr. Biol. 12(10), 2002
PMID: 12015120
The what and why of binding: the modeler's perspective.
von der Malsburg C., Neuron 24(1), 1999
PMID: 10677030
von der Malsburg C., Neuron 24(1), 1999
PMID: 10677030
Laws of organization in perceptual forms
Wertheimer M., 1936
Wertheimer M., 1936
Bayesian decision theory and psychophysics
Yuille AL, Bütlhoff HH., 1996
Yuille AL, Bütlhoff HH., 1996
Export
Markieren/ Markierung löschen
Markierte Publikationen
Web of Science
Dieser Datensatz im Web of Science®Quellen
PMID: 16441199
PubMed | Europe PMC
Suchen in